Roulette William Smith, Ph.D.
Institute
for Postgraduate Interdisciplinary Studies
Palo Alto, CA 94306-0846 USA
E-Mail:
najms@postgraduate-interdisciplinary-studies.org
E-Mail: najms@humanized-technologies.com
Revised Version of a Presentation to the
4th International Conference on
Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies
Baiyun, Guangzhou (CHINA) – September 24th-26th,
2007
Abstract
[1]
Transpersonal psychology subsumes many areas, although issues pertaining to
‘consciousness’
remain a central theme underlying many studies and reports. Examples include
classic studies
of consciousness and altered states of consciousness, meditation and
mindfulness, shamanism
and mind-altering substances, spirituality, and personal transformations.
Significantly,
philosophers and neuroscientists now are making substantial inroads into the
biological and
molecular basis for consciousness.
[2]
Philosophy Professor David J. Chalmers suggests that the challenges of
consciousness should
be dichotomized into “easy problems,” on the one hand, and the “hard”
or “really hard problem,”
on the other hand (1995). According to Chalmers:
“The easy
problems of consciousness are those that seem directly
susceptible to the standard methods of cognitive science, whereby a
phenomenon is explained in terms of computational or neural
mechanisms. … The easy problems of consciousness include
those of
explaining the following phenomena:
Chalmers then states
that: “The really hard problem of consciousness is the problem of
experience. When we think and perceive, there is a whir of
information-processing, but there is also a subjective aspect. As Nagel (1974)
has put it, there is something it is like to be a conscious organism. This
subjective aspect is experience. When we see, for example, we experience visual
sensations: the felt quality of redness, the experience of dark and light, the
quality of depth in a visual field. Other experiences go along with perception
in different modalities: the sound of a clarinet, the smell of mothballs. Then
there are bodily
sensations, from
pains to orgasms; mental images that are conjured
up internally; the felt quality of emotion, and the experience of a
stream of conscious thought. What unites all of these states is that
there is something it is like to be in them. All of them are states of
experience.” (p. 201)
[3]
This report focuses on a third type of difficult problems; to wit, three
intriguing problems of
awareness, belief and reality. These problems
occasionally appear in clinical clients who
generally are not considered appropriate for research or laboratory
investigations of
consciousness. Their circumstances, challenges and problems ultimately may
prove to be far
more significant to researchers, especially if their realities, when
compared to their experiences,
contribute to the elucidation and explication of the onset and formation of beliefs.
[4]
One type of problem associated with reality is identified in persons who need
help or assistance,
and yet those persons have absolutely no knowledge or cognitive understanding
of their needs
for assistance. There can be no mistake that these persons possess
consciousness. Nor is
there any doubt that they possess personal senses of realities. Yet they often
misunderstand
more than they understand. They make mistakes and break things. They would
rather replace a
broken item rather than repair it. Their problems are classified under the
rubric of “unknowing
neediness.”
[5]
The second type of problem is identified in persons who constantly seek help or
assistance, and
yet they have absolutely no need for assistance. These persons consume enormous
quantities
of attention needlessly. Their behaviors are costly and chaotic. Their problems
are associated
with “worried wellness.” The worried well also misunderstand more than
they understand on
occasions. Interestingly, their problems often are relegated to third-party
insurance providers
that decide whether or not to pay claims. In the end, neither these persons,
their professional
healthcare providers, nor others are well-served by an underlying dysfunctional
healthcare
system unresponsive to fundamental needs.
[6]
The third type of problem perhaps is even more important at a practical level.
The underlying
clinical challenges point to issues of awareness – apart from
beliefs, experiences and realities –
in scholars, clinicians and potential clients. In other words, Chalmers’ really
hard problem
overlooks a transpersonal meta-issue of awareness in clinicians,
scientists, and, their subjects
and co-researchers. This meta-issue occasionally is manifested in ‘experimenter
effects’,
experimental bias, poor experimental design, and, failures in logic and “scientific
(and scholarly)
‘common sense’” (Smith, 1983; Smith, 2006b; Smith, in preparation).
[7]
Although differences in reality and perceived experiences may be
minor or subtle in most
persons, those differences may be quite profound in both the unknowingly needy
and worried
well. This report focuses on a specific subgroup of persons who are unknowingly
needy and/or
worried well. In particular, the focus is on persons who do not have
‘common sense’,1 and rarely
1 Lacking common sense,
having no common sense, and aberrant common sense all are terms referring to
persons’ ways of thinking that differ from the ways of thinking in their
cultures, groups or herds. In other words, Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William
Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
seek clinical
support. Attention is directed to the extremely hard problem of
consciousness involving:
documenting and
treating unknowing neediness and worried wellness. Persons who do not have
“common sense” occasionally may not understand, may misunderstand,
cannot understand, or may be out of touch with their
consciousness and/or realities.
[8] Why focus on
common sense and the lack of common sense? The present studies of common sense
date back to the 1970s and 1980s. During the 1970s, Smith (1971) flirted with
artificial intelligence aspects of common sense.2 Then, quite
fortuitously in 1985, 9 young elementary school students were observed who did
not have “common sense.” This determination was based on their responses to
mathematics questions and problems, and other aberrant personal behaviors. The
students were enrolled in grades 3 to 6 in a Sunnyvale, California (USA)
elementary school Mathematics Laboratory. The mathematics laboratory provided
remedial support for students performing poorly on mathematics tasks, and
provided enrichment tasks and activities for “gifted” students.
[9] The Mathematics
Laboratory was located in one section of the School Library. In a fortuitous
conversation with the school librarian, she revealed that those same 9 students
also performed poorly on reading tasks. These observations were reported to the
school principal who then recommended that these matters be discussed in
parent-teacher conferences. Those parent-teacher conferences revealed that for
each of those 9 students, one or both parents were uniformly “negative.” Those
parents simply did not (and possibly could not) say anything good, positive or
commendable about their child (Smith, 1986; Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; cf.
Smith, 1971). After extensive historical and biographical research, the
phenomenon of “aberrant” common sense3 associated with parental
negativism was found to be widespread and universal, though not appreciated in
education, psychology, medicine, other social sciences, or any clinical
professions (Smith, 1992). The term “psychoviruses” then was introduced to
possibly explain the transmission of non-genetic information leading to the
evolution and development of aberrations in common sense and in other
psychosocial disorders or dysfunctions.
[10] Psychoviruses
are snippets of infectious, non-genetic information which interfere in
“normal” cognitive development. Those snippets of information indirectly may
lead to changes in DNA in brain. Psychovirus effects can be especially profound
in children between their births and approximately age six. Children appear to
be especially susceptible to adverse effects during the “terrible twos” and
shortly afterward. Situational effects also can produce psychoviruses and
those persons have
and use personalized styles of “sense” which do not comport with the common
styles of
“sense” used by peers, within herds or other groups.
2 This report does not review artificial intelligence issues
pertaining to common sense and consciousness. Rather,
the focus in this report is on “transpersonal” aspects of common sense in
humans.
3 Throughout this report we use “aberrant” to connote sufficiently
unusual manifestations of a phenomenon which
on the surface may appear usual or “normal.” Upon finer grained analyses – and
particularly in selected situations –
the underlying phenomena may be profoundly different, though not worthy of the
labels “abnormal” or “disabled.”
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
psychoviral
responses (Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992; Reuters, 2006; Christakis and
Fowler, 2007). Although the concept of psychoviruses predates computer viruses,
the computer virus metaphor is appropriate. Psychoviruses differ from memes
(Dawkins, 1976) insofar as the gene–meme metaphor cannot explain many clinical,
laboratory or molecular findings which psychoviruses can explain. The gene–meme
metaphor also cannot explain evolutionary findings associated with a
“tripartite” model of evolution (Smith, 2005a; Smith, 2005b; Smith, 2006a;
Smith, 2006b; Smith, in preparation). Not insignificantly, the notion of
psychoviruses portends a potential companion notion of ‘psychovaccines’.
[11]
A possible molecular and evolutionary basis for common sense was investigated
during the past
three years (Smith, 2004a; Smith, 2004b; Smith, 2004c; Smith, 2007a; Smith,
2007b; Smith,
2007c). This includes studies of common sense in more than 41 cultures
worldwide (see Table
1; cf. Taormina, 2006).
[12]
As a microcosm, one component of the common sense research focuses on the
evolution and
development of common sense in post-World War II Germans, Jewish Holocaust
survivors
residing in the USA, and Jewish Holocaust survivors residing in Israel.4
These groups were
selected because much of World War II history and its consequences are amply
documented
and archived. Even if common sense in Germans and Jewish persons differed
before World
War II, one hypothetically5 should not expect statistically
significant differences among Jewish
Holocaust survivors in Israel and the USA. Thus, the underlying design provides
important,
though not-too-rigorous, controls in these exploratory studies.
[13]
Preliminary evidence suggests three divergent strands of common sense
associated with these
three subpopulations. War and other trauma also appear to contribute generally
to divergences
in common sense elsewhere6 – resulting from genocide,
ethnic-cleansing, other crimes against
humanity, and other specific traumatic events. A few examples include:
4 Although Holocaust victims
included many persons and groups other than Jewish persons, the decision to
focus only on Jewish persons is based on practical considerations (e.g.,
availability of archives and documents, identifiable survivors and their
offspring, access, etc.). 5 This is a “null” hypothesis. In actual
fact, one should not be too surprised if there are significant divergences in
common sense in Jewish Holocaust survivors in Israel and the USA. Environment,
culture, government and other factors may contribute to those divergences. 6
In all instances of divergences in common sense cited in this report,
attributions of responsibilities or “blame” are avoided, even though blame and
causality may have relevance. Rather, the sole focus is on phenomena underlying
common sense, aberrant common sense, and changes in common sense. A goal is to
understand how evolutionary, molecular, developmental, situational and
clinical events may contribute to the explication of common sense and
changes in common sense. 7 Whereas Boccaccio’s writings were
diachronic, Tuchman’s approach was more synchronic.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Even during the 4th
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies
and Psychotherapies, strife and violence among monks and the military in
Myanmar are a
replay of the violent 1988 clashes between students and the military in Burma.
More recently,
wild firestorms in southern California traumatized many persons after more than
2300 homes
and structures were burned to the ground and more than 500,000 persons had to
be evacuated.
Thus, if instances of war and trauma contribute to divergences in common sense
in individuals
and groups/herds, then studying processes and dynamics underlying experiences,
belief
formation, reality, and awareness may have value – especially if they have
profound
psychological, social, political and moral implications requiring universal
caveats emptor (Smith,
1986; Smith, 1987; Smith, 1992).
[14]
The extremely hard problem discussed in this report is not
associated with separate general
cultural experiences per se. For example, divergent differences in
common sense in Germans,
8 Examples include: “Parable
of the Old Men and the Young”; “The Dead-Beat”; “Mental Cases”; “Arms and the
Boy”; and “Conscious.”
Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Jewish Holocaust
survivors residing in the USA, or Jewish Holocaust survivors residing in Israel
per se are not considered. Rather, an immediate challenge concerns
separate realities among a unique and very small sample of German
persons9 who lack common sense (cf. Smith, 1986; Smith, 1987; Smith,
1988). Their common sense (or absence in common sense) does not comport with
mainstream common sense in Germany. The present in situ10
phenomenological study examines theoretical, philosophical (including moral and
ethical), methodological, economic, developmental, epidemiological and clinical
issues associated with this small sample of unknowingly needy and worried well
persons.
[15] Insofar as
‘common’ sense in humans develops between birth and approximately age six years
old (Smith, 1988; cf. Fulghum, 1986/2004), the extremely hard problem
specifically includes concerns for the extremely difficult scholarly and
clinical challenges of nurturing common sense skills in pre-school and
elementary school aged children who lack common sense. Because the propositi
and co-researchers in this study are adults, the focus is expanded to include
high school students, post-baccalaureate young adults, and older adults – none
of whom may have common sense.11 Older adults who lack common sense
may provide clues to boundaries and limitations regarding the intractability of
possible clinical treatments and therapeutic responses. Older adults also may
shed light on the long-term stability of common sense. A central theme guiding
this research is whether one can help these unknowingly needy persons. Is
providing assistance and nurturing change a lost cause? If DNA plasticity is
affirmed based on a hypothesis that DNA is the repository of long-term memories
(Smith, 1979; Smith, 2003b; cf. Exhibit 1), what combination of molecular,
psychopharmacological and/or therapeutic approaches can optimize therapeutic
responses, if at all – and at what ages?
[16] On a broader
scale, one is reminded that war often is a target and object of
many (military, political, game-theoretic and other) decisions and studies.
Research on common sense and aberrant common sense suggests that stability in
common sense may be a concrete benefit of peace. Examples of war and traumatic
situations possibly contributing to divergences in common sense were cited
earlier. The potential for homogeneity in common sense and possible
reduction in the prevalence of instances of aberrant common sense could be
one benefit and objective. Other possible benefits include reductions in costs
associated with clinical, diagnostic and therapeutic activities, as well as
societal costs associated with unforeseeable consequences of divergences in
common sense – including many of the types of traumatic events cited above. In
other words, micro- and macro-economic aspects of common sense must be factored
into the extremely hard problem, particularly insofar as rational thinking and
behaviors become economic issues. An apt analogy might be Ignaz Semmelweis’
observations about hospital sanitation when coupled with recent concerns about
methicillin-resistant (or, more accurately, multi-resistant) staphylococcus
aureus (MRSA; cf. Klevens et al., 2007). War and trauma may contribute to
messy, unhealthy and costly multi-resistant divergences in common
9 That the propositi in this
study are Germans may be purely coincidental – an accident of circumstances.
Retrospective analyses of other persons lacking common sense from the larger
database affirm many of the findings in this report. 10 The term in
situ is derived from Latin and means “in the situation.” 11
Throughout this report, we use aberrant common sense, having no common sense,
and lacking common sense interchangeably. Again, we are challenged to find
appropriate nomenclature which is sufficiently descriptive of underlying
cognitive processes. In alleging that persons have no common sense, there is no
intent to imply that those persons have no sense. Quite to the contrary, they
merely lack an appreciation for others’ sense within their herd, clan or
culture, and how their own behaviors do not comport with others’ common
or communal sense.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
sense – including
further aberrations in common sense and possibly leading to additional war and
“terrorism.” The hidden micro- and macro-psychological and economic costs
associated with chaos, war, terrorism, trauma, and stress-related disorders
must take front and center stage.
[17] At an
epistemological level, this report indirectly examines consequences of
professional failures to elucidate and explicate clinical aspects of
negativism, common sense, unknowing neediness and worried wellness in clinical
psychology, psychiatry, medicine, the neurosciences, and clinical social work.
Insofar as negativism and aberrant common sense often are manifested as chaotic
thinking and behaviors, occasional costly and harmful consequences, and in
other forms of inappropriate and complex human dynamics, the absence of a
negative personality disorder (cf. Millon, 1981) and aberrant common sense in
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) IV-R TR
and various versions of the International Statistical Classification of
Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD) – 9 and 10 now must be
redressed. The term “common sense” does not appear in the DSM IV-R TR, any
version of the ICD, or in any known clinical or professional textbook in
medicine, clinical psychology, or clinical social work.12 Yet,
disorders of negativism and common sense pose special challenges because they
mimic personality psychopathology as well as sociopathology due to their impact
on and consequences for others. Moreover, professional trends in most health,
health care and public health systems require that clients seek professional
assistance rather than professionals seeking out the unknowingly needy.
Unknowing neediness simply is not on the professional ‘radar’ – and especially
in disorders of common sense. Persons who lack common sense generally do not
seek help and require novel interventions. Worried wellness usually falls
within the purview of health insurers largely because of economic
considerations. There is little consideration for experiential aspects within a
client’s reality, within professional realities, or within the intersection of
those realities.
[18] In summary, a
truly difficult problem for consciousness research is brought to light in
studies of common sense and aberrant common sense. This problem becomes even
more complex when consciousness issues underlying experimenter-subject and
clinician-client dyads become a part of the challenge. Assessments of awareness
and need then become a central part of the equation.
Introduction
[19] When
Professors Cyrus and Magdalena Lee issued their call for papers for the 4th
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and
Psychotherapies, the plan was to submit a manuscript discussing recent and
preliminary results on divergences in “common sense” obtained during the past
two years. A two month retreat in Germany was organized with a goal to assemble
and analyze data related to post-World War II divergences in common sense in
Germans, and Jewish Holocaust survivors in the USA and Israel. Insofar as a previous
12 Although many features of
aberrant common sense mimic the borderline personality, it is sufficiently
different and deserves its own Axis II designation. The ICD-10 does include two
classifications (that is, F94.8 – Other childhood disorders of social
functioning; and, F94.9 – Childhood disorder of social functioning,
unspecified) which could subsume some commonsense-related issues, although
those categories do not take into account adult-related matters. Those
classifications also do not capture the cognitive aspects of the putative
disorder. General anxiety disorders in adults (ICD-10 – F41.1; ICD-9
– 300.02) also are presenting symptoms in aberrant common sense, yet these
disorders fail to characterize the spectrum of findings.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
presentation at the
3rd International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal
Psychologies and Psychotherapies dealt with new perspectives on evolution
and their implications for nurturance and the transpersonal
(Smith, 2005), a goal this year was to show, at least theoretically, that
“common sense” and its divergences have a biological basis in humans consistent
with the far-reaching “tripartite” (that is, three part) theory of evolution. A
working hypothesis is that common sense generally is encoded in non-proteomic
regions of the DNA genome. It is nurtured between the time of birth and roughly
age six, and is not genetic per se. To the extent that common sense may
represent herd behavior, a reasonable goal is to determine if there are common
encodings for common sense within herds, cultures, etc. Just as a ‘genetic
code’ facilitates consistent protein production based on genes in the proteome,
a common non-proteomic encoding scheme may underlie long-term memory
mechanisms. Ultimately, changing Guanine*Cytosine::Adenine*Thymine13
ratios in selected regions of brain might serve as crude markers for assessing
common sense traits and components. These crude markers could lead to the first
serious efforts aimed at distinguishing common and unique consequences of
nurturance – and consciousness. This long-term research approach also can
demonstrate breadth, depth and richness in the tripartite theory of evolution.
That is, the theory is sufficiently powerful to capture and explain some of the
most elemental forms of the human experiences – and even at a molecular level.
[20] Common sense
is one of those elemental experiences. Most persons use the term common sense
in their everyday lives, yet as noted above, there are very few discussions of
the psychology of common sense in the literature. Despite its use, without an operational
definition, one may never truly “know” what is meant by the term common sense
within any herd or cultural context. Even when persons are asked to define
common sense, they often encounter considerable difficulties. Indeed, initial
interest in German common sense (in contrast to common sense in Germany) arose
in the late-1980s in conversations with friends and professional colleagues in
Munich, Germany. When discussing then extant research on common sense in young
children (Smith, 1986; Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988), there was no uniform
agreement regarding an appropriate German term for common sense. There was
general agreement that gesundermenschenverstand best represents the
notion of common sense. Moreover, because there is general agreement that Gezond
verstand is the Dutch expression for common sense and because of
similarities in Dutch and German languages, it is reasonable to assume that gesundermenschenverstand
is an appropriate representation of what generally is regarded as common sense.
Table 1 lists many representations of the term common sense in different
languages.
[21] The challenge
of understanding common sense is far more complex than one of determining
definitions or common terminologies (cf. Table 1). Psychotherapist Salamin
Alphonse (at the 4th International Conference on
Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies, personal
communication) notes that while bon sens may represent a correct
dictionary translation of the term into French, sens commun or sens
pratique may be more appropriate. The French term sens pratique (cf.
Bourdieu, 1998; Geertz, 1983; Robinson, 1983)14 may provide a clue
to an essential element in common sense; to wit, the importance of some
13 Hereafter designated G*C::A*T.
14 Clifford Geertz defines common sense as a form of ‘local
knowledge’ (Geertz, 1983); to wit, cultural language that forms the basis for
all agreements and is implied but not necessarily written. His emphasis is on
knowledge not underlying cognitive processes. Pierre Bourdieu (cf. <http://en.wikipediai.org/wiki/Pierre_Bourdieu> and <http://fr.wikipedia.org/wiki/Pierre_Bourdieu>) defines sens
pratique in terms of fields, habitus and doxa.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
underlying
practical and socially conforming cognitive (memory and mental
processing) activities within groups, herds and/or cultures.15
This would support occasional claims that a person is “book smart,” yet has no
common sense.
[22] Table 1
provides a clue to another important aspect of common sense. The traditional
Chinese idiographic characters and Sanskrit terms for common sense point to
possible long-term and evolutionary aspects underlying common sense. These
representations underscore the importance of distinguishing and disambiguating nature
and nurture in discussions of common sense. Although many scholarly
pronouncements on common sense by western philosophers and religious scholars
may have originated in medicine during Middle Ages and Renaissance (Mullooly,
2003; Mullooly, 2006), the Chinese and Indian linguistic traditions clearly
indicate much earlier uses of the concept / notion. There also is evidence of
Aboriginal persons in Australia having historical and cultural notions of
common sense.16 Furthermore, when the naturalistic report of a
battle between a herd water buffalo and pride of lions is considered
(Schlosberg and Budzinski, 2004), it is evident that both biological (that is,
nature) and herd / cultural (that is, nurturance, development and adaptation)
components are important.
A Role for Evolution in Common Sense and A Role for
Common Sense in Evolution
[23] A tripartite
theory of evolution (Smith, 2005b; Smith, 2006a; Smith, 2006a; Smith, in
preparation) differs from Charles Darwin’s theory insofar as the tripartite
theory has three unique components. The first component (A) subsumes all
of Charles Darwin’s ideas. In other words, Darwin’s theory is necessary,
though not sufficient, to explain human evolution. The two remaining components
in the tripartite theory are: B) in utero experiences and
possible consequences in later life related to those in utero
experiences (that is, intrauterine events and “experiences” between mother and
child taking place in a woman’s womb during pregnancy; possible transfers of
‘soulful’ and nurturing information in utero, and, possible long-term
consequences of drugs, addictions, methylations/imprinting and the intrauterine
environment; cf. Verny and Kelly, 1981/1983; Barker et al., 1989; cf. Coles,
1990; Haig, 1996; Forsen et al., 2000; Killian et al., 2000; Barker, 2001;
Godfrey and Barker, 2001; Eriksson et al., 2001; Reik and Walter, 2001; Verny
and Weintraub, 2002; Barker, 2002; Barker, 2003a; Barker, 2003b; Bihl, 2003;
DiPietro, 2006; Dolinoy, Huang and Jirtle, 2007); and, C) DNA is the
repository of long-term memories in brain and the immune system (Smith, 1979;
see Exhibit 1). An abundance of clinical, epidemiological, experimental and
theoretical evidence suggests that changes in DNA occur dynamically largely in
non-proteomic regions of the genome (Smith, in preparation). Preliminary
evidence from a variety of sources suggests that many of those DNA changes in
brain may convert adenine*thymine-rich regions to guanine*cytosine-richer
regions, possibly accompanied by methylation events. In immune memories,
recombinations and rearrangements in immunoglobulin hypervariable genes
represent an established mechanism (Tonegawa et al., 1978; Sakano et al.,
1979).
[24]
15 I especially am grateful to
Jutta Thompson, Greg Andonian, Gerard De Zeeuw, Salamin Alphonse, Fei Zi, Ming
Lee, John Clemens, Julio Vidaurrazaga, Byron Marshall, James Stasheff, Michael
Eisenstadt and others for assisting me in sorting out the importance of various
cognitive components underlying common sense and aberrant common sense. These
components include memory, culture / herd, processing, problem-solving,
mistakes and error processing, etc. 16 At this time, it is not known
if any Aboriginal notions of common sense are consistent with ‘dream time’.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Other memory
mechanisms associated with C include mutable loci and transpositions
(McClintock, 1950; Smith, 1979), mirror neuron activities (e.g., imitation and
grasping of intentions; di Pellegrino et al., 1992; Fadiga et al., 1995;
Rizzolati et al., 1996a; Rizzolati et al., 1996b; Gallese et al., 1996; Grafton
et al., 1999; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Arbib et al., 2000; Ramachandran, 2000;
Iacoboni et al., 2001; Iacoboni et al., 2005), and psychovirus actions (Smith,
1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992). In addition, new transmissible and epigenetic
memory mechanisms (e.g., autotoxicity, autovirulence, context-specificity,
‘hit-and-run’ and ‘beneath-theradar’ transmissible infections; Smith, 1983;
Smith, 1984; Smith, 2003a) may contribute to autoimmune, psychosomatic and
other psycho-immuno-neurological axis disorders and syndromes.
[25] What generally
distinguishes A from B and C is the forms of information
transmitted and how that information is reproduced and replicated. The
replication and transmission of molecular information associated with A
primarily is genetic. Although Darwinian evolution presumably can accommodate
the transmission of substituent particles (for example, prions and other
autotoxins; transposons, microRNAs, snRNPs, and other autovirions; and other
potential generators of diversity [Smith, 1984; Smith, 1989]), psychoviruses
and mirror neuron actions (e.g., imitation and grasping of intentions) are more
difficult to reconcile. Components B and C generally cannot be
explained in Darwinian evolutionary terms. B and C can explain
Lamarckian evolution and more – including common sense. C identifies
other evolutionary advantages often overlooked in Darwinian evolution –
including aging and different economic benefits (e.g., volunteerism and
philanthropy). This tripartite evolutionary perspective may generalize to marsupial
mammals, too (cf. Killian et al., 2000). Marsupials include opossums in the
Earth’s northern and southern hemispheres, along with an extraordinary variety
of other marsupials mostly in the southern hemisphere (for example, kangaroos,
wallabies, koala bears, wombats, and even the Tasmanian devil and thylocine;
see <http://www.pbase.com/mr2c280/australia_mammals>).
[26] That there may
be a molecular basis for common sense and that there is a role for evolution in
common sense, is not at all obvious. Some scholars may be inclined view common
sense diachronously (that is, of, relating to, or dealing with phenomena
[as of language or culture] as they occur or change over a period of time
prospectively and/or cumulatively) or synchronously (that is, chronological
arrangement of historical events and personages so as to indicate coincidence
or coexistence; retrospectively and/or historically). These dichotomies,
dualities, oppositions and distinctions may be artificial and simplistic – and
represent instances of descriptive – structuralism and its inadequacies
(Smith, 1983). Descriptive-structuralism is unlikely to shed any light on a
possible molecular basis for common sense.
[27] A second
possible approach to explicating common sense might involve heuristic –
functionalism (Smith, 1983). One’s views of common sense must appreciate
diachronous and synchronous (i.e., descriptive) details. Yet, an underlying
appreciation for molecular and biological (i.e., functional) processes also is
in order. Those processes may provide clues to broad molecular elements,
even though those processes remain to be more fully explicated. As noted
earlier, common sense possibly is encoded in non-proteomic portions of the
genome. To the extent that this possibility is affirmed, an immediate challenge
and long-term goal may be to ascertain whether there really are “common”
elements underlying brain activities – and especially in regard to
consciousness and common sense. In other words, does the term “common” in
common sense have relevance at a molecular and non-proteomic level?
[28] This report
reveals a third possible approach to explicating common sense. After planning a
two month scholarly retreat in Germany in order to complete a report for the 4th
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies
and Psychotherapies, those best laid plans were derailed because of
emergent chaos in the German household. The planned report not only would
provide preliminary evidence of three divergent strands in common sense emerging
from post-World War II, the report would have considered biological,
evolutionary and developmental advantages of common sense to survivors of World
War II. In the end, aberrant common sense was determined to be the source of
the chaos. This third approach emerged from that chaos. In situ
observations of aberrant commonsense17 behaviors were
extraordinarily rich, informative and invaluable, and, represent a unique and
fortuitous embodiment of logistic reasoning (Smith, 1983). By viewing
aberrant common sense in real-time, one now may be able to understand more
about both common sense and aberrant common sense.
[29] In summary,
preliminary findings related to the evolution of common sense in generations of
post-World War II Germans and Holocaust survivors, when coupled with logistic
reasoning about aberrant common sense, possibly can provide a glimpse into
biological, developmental and evolutionary mechanisms underlying common sense
and aberrant common sense. These findings also will support using common sense
as a concrete marker when building a solid foundation in peace studies.
Finally, the findings now propel the ‘transpersonal realm’ into virgin and
uncharted territories in genomic studies and neurosciences.
[30] Logistic
reasoning also provides clues to needs for proactive and anticipatory
strategies aimed at avoiding divergences in common sense. For example,
increasing numbers of mothers now serve in the USA military services. This
phenomenon also is occurring in other nations. Estimates of USA women soldiers
serving in the Iraq war now exceed 170,000 tours in duty. Women also comprise
approximately 10% of soldiers assigned to the war in Iraq (<http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=14964676>; Norris, 2007). Women
in the USA military services also have higher prevalence rates of
post-traumatic stress disorders (PTSD) than their male counterparts (Norris and
Hillard, 2007; Milliken, Auchterlonie and Hoge, 2007). Having women serve in
the military is deemed a social objective aimed at reducing gender inequities
and providing human rights consistent with the United States Constitution. Yet,
many of those military mothers are being separated from their young children
during those children’s formative years when common sense is developing. Those
children also could be exposed to psychoviruses or other situational stresses
from other non-parental and non-familial sources.
[31] Military
service women of childbearing ages, and who may have experienced PTSD, pose a
second challenge. Their PTSD could lead to aberrant nurturance of any offspring
subsequent to their diagnosis of PTSD, thereby causing other divergences in
common sense. Ultimately, an increase in prevalence rates of aberrant common
sense may be a consequence of the shortsightedness in USA military strategies
regarding the lack of strategic planning for long-term
17 Throughout this report,
common sense is used as a noun, and commonsense is used as an adjective.
Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
consequences of
childbearing women in the military (cf. Montagne, 2007; Norris, 2007; Milliken,
Auchterlonie and Hoge, 2007; also see <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=14964676>). Any aberrations in
common sense may be viewed as concrete markers and indicators of the
ill-thought and ill-advised prosecution of the wars in Afghanistan and Iraq.
Even more important, the national caveats emptor in regard to “terrorism” are
profound (cf. Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992; Smith, 2002). Insofar as
these wars allegedly are responses to terrorism, evidence of excessive
humiliation and torture (at Abu Ghraib prison in Iraq, CIA extraordinary
rendition sites worldwide, and the US detention facility at Guantanamo Bay,
Cuba), terrorism could beget further torture and terrorism – contributing to a
cycle of divergent, divergences in common sense. Similar cycles of terrorism
are evident in Russia where Chechnyan terrorists become even more emboldened by
Russian responses to Chechnyan terrorist attacks.
[32]
This proactive scenario should cause one to pause and consider what clinicians,
scholars,
transpersonalists, military planners and others should recommend and practice
if peace,
common sense and sanity are to be preserved – and if one’s offspring (and
others) are to enjoy
long-term benefits of peace. There also is a need for concrete
technologies (in terms of reliability
and validity) to assess common sense and its aberrations – and, by inference,
peace. It now is
time to invent and develop non-invasive technologies to assess DNA changes –
and to correlate
DNA dynamics with common sense, peace and other clinical entities.
[33]
Parenthetically, in citing the proactive consequences of the war on future
manifestations of
common sense in the USA and elsewhere, it also is worth mentioning a
little-noted proactive
aspect and consequence of China’s ‘one child” policies on population control.
There is little
evidence that the forefathers of this policy weighed its consequences and
implications for the
spread of HIV/AIDS. This example is particularly important because of a
question from a young
female university student at the 25 September 2007 “Going Along with Professors
– Speakers of
the World” Forum at the International Hall in one of university of Guangzhou’s
10-university
center complex. The event was co-hosted by the Guangzhou University of Chinese
Medicine.
The woman requested professors’ views on “one-night (sexual) stands” (cf.
Wilson, 2007).
[34]
The young woman’s question was extraordinarily important insofar as the spread
of HIV in
university communities can have devastating long-term consequences. Her
question also was
important because of the relative imbalance in the ratio of males to females
caused by China’s
one-child policy. The transmission of other sexually transmissible diseases is
no less important
(cf. Moss et al., 2007). Most persons overlook one extremely important
epidemiologic fact about
HIV, lentiviruses (in general), and other transmissible agents causing slowly
progressive
diseases (for example, prions). In all instances, the quantity of the virus is
inversely correlated
with the profoundness of disease within an individual and within the herd
(Smith, 1984; Smith,
1994; Smith, 2001; cf. Kelley et al., 2007). As virus titers increase (that is,
as virus “load”
increases), then incubation periods become shorter. Over the long-term, as
virus titers increase,
manifestations of diseases become more profound. This finding has special
significance in
physical islands and social islands because virus titers sometimes can increase
exponentially –
both within individuals and herds. Perhaps more important, all agents causing
slowly
progressive processes comport with as many as eleven ‘near-axiomatic’
features – including the
inverse proportionality rule and an ‘island’ hypothesis (Smith, 1994).
[35] The ‘island’
hypothesis states that prevalence rates for infectious agents causing slowly
progressive diseases and the profoundness of those associated diseases generally
are greater in island environments. College campuses often are social islands
with many unknowingly needy clients (cf. Wilson, 2007). These factors, taken
together, reveal increased risks for women in China (that is, a social island
confounded by the one-child policy) to receive higher titers of HIV than their
male counterparts – at least initially. To the extent that there are fewer
female sexual partners available in the society, increased titers and
infections rates then can shift to males. Hence, if sexual activities are
carried out without consideration for others, the prevalence of HIV and
opportunistic infections could continue to rise exponentially – and
dynamically, shifting disproportionately among females and males. In short,
common sense, common knowledge, and evolutionary considerations dictate
that human sexual predilections take into account HIV/AIDS, other sexually
transmitted diseases, and socio-politico dynamics. By this logic, “one night”
stands, at this time in history, may be regarded as a form of aberrant common
sense.
[36] Overall, the
spread of HIV in humans is nicely illustrated in the Benetton photo
essay appended near the end of this paper (Exhibit 2). The particular issue of
the Benetton Colors Magazine appeared in 2000 – just in time for the XIII
International AIDS Conference in Durham, South Africa. At that time, the
spread of HIV/AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa was thought to represent the worse
case scenario in this dread pandemic. The alarming changing prevalence rates of
HIV/AIDS in India and China now should give pause to all Chinese nationals –
and especially to university and college students (cf. Wilson, 2007). Along
with the spread of HIV/AIDS, one can anticipate an increase in the prevalence
rates and varieties of other infectious diseases – and especially sexually
transmitted diseases.
[37] Before leaving
the issue of HIV/AIDS, it is important to stress the underlying common sense and
common knowledge implications (see Footnote 46). Knowledge about HIV/AIDS,
other sexually transmitted diseases, and opportunistic pathogens must become
common knowledge, in addition to the common sense issues regarding
transmissibility of infectious pathogens. Cooperation is a central tenet in
common sense. This is in contrast to reasoning used by virtually all persons
with aberrant common sense. All other things being equal, the ways and actions
of persons who lack common sense focus solely on their own ways and actions –
and not the ways and actions of the herd. The last two graphics in this report
are taken from an elementary school morality education textbook entitled “SHOGAKU
DŌU TŌKU/どうとく(Morality /
The way of virtue) for 1st, 2nd, and 3rd
grades” (Umiuchi et al., circa 1967; Exhibit 4). They are excellent
illustrations of both cooperation and common sense.18 Similar crisp
and clear, “commonsense” instructions about cooperation and considerations
for others were provided as animated passenger information on Japan
Airlines (JAL) flights to the 4th International Conference
on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies. Another
commonsense example from recent Japanese literature is Moriko Shinju’s Mottainai
Grandma comic series illustrating the inappropriateness of waste (Shinju,
2004/2004; Kestenbaum, 2007). Shinju’s underlying message is the common adage
“waste not; want not.”
18 Unique in the second lesson
is the exceptional step (the last panel on the bottom left) taken by the fox
character to teach others what it (that is, the fox) learned from the bear.
This step generally is not seen in lesson plans in schools in the USA. The
example also illustrates cultural differences in common sense.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
An In Situ Phenomenological Analysis of
Aberrant Common Sense
[38] As mentioned
earlier, a two month scholarly retreat was planned to complete a manuscript for
this 4th International Conference on Humanistic and
Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies. Those plans were disrupted
for elusive reasons which remain difficult to fully understand. During the
retreat in Germany, glaring, costly and potentially harmful examples of
aberrant common sense were continually encountered. The remainder of this
presentation focuses on an in situ phenomenological study19 and
analysis of aberrant common sense during that two month period. The situation
was particularly interesting because of the confluence in emerging concerns for
“illness,” “aberrancy,” “health,” “wellness” and “helping.” Regarding helping,
the focus in this in situ study is on unknowing neediness (in persons
lacking common sense), and not the worried well. This is not to conclude that
persons lacking common sense cannot be among the worried well. Indeed,
Proposita “D” (see below) often presents as a hopelessly worried well client.20
Propositi –
co-researchers in this study21
[39]
The propositi in this report include a divorced middle-aged mother (Proposita
“A”) and her two
young adult, interracial22 daughters Propositi “B” and “C”. The
investigator has known Proposita
“A” since July 2004. He met Propositi “B” and “C” in March 2005. Other
propositi include two
somewhat elderly next door neighbors (Proposita “Y” and Proposita “Z”) whom the
he also met
in March 2005. Propositi “Y” and “Z” are unrelated.
[40]
Of these propositi, Proposita “C” clearly lacks common sense. This was
immediately apparent in
March 2005. Aberrant common sense in Propositi “Y” and “Z” became evident in
2006. Their
aberrancies in common sense are somewhat peripheral to the present study,
except insofar as
their behaviors initially sparked concerns about potentially high prevalence
rates of aberrant
common sense in post-World War II Germany. Propositi “A,” “B,” “C,” “Y,” and
“Z” are German
citizens residing in a large urban city in Germany. Proposita “A” was diagnosed
with aberrant
common sense in May 2007 midway through this in situ study. There is no
evidence that
Proposita “B” has any aberration in common sense, although indirect evidence
suggests that her
father Propositus “X” lacks common sense.
[41]
A divorced middle-aged mother (Proposita “D”) is known to this investigator
since 1972. Her
aberrant common sense was diagnosed in 1985 – shortly after it was reported
that transmissible
negativism and aberrant common sense represent important clinical entities.
Prior to 1985,
19 Phenomenological studies in
psychology and other social sciences are not new (cf. Braud and Anderson,
1998). This study is unique insofar as its in situ component is more
akin to ethnographic research in anthropology. 20 Interest in the
unknowingly needy and worried well is derived from the classic adage / paradigm
about the dichotomization of knowledge and action (see Endnote after the
References). 21 The word “propositi” is the plural of proposita
(female) and/or propositus (male). These terms refer to designated
persons in a pedigree or family tree. 22 Interracial, interfaith and
interethnic relationships are cited because of past findings associated with
propositi who lack common sense (Smith, 1992; Smith, 2004c). Of 37 propositi
marriages and divorces, 24 of those marriages were interracial and 1 was an
interfaith marriage (67.6%). Also, of those 37 propositi marriages, 16 involved
2nd marriages (43.2%), and 1 involved a 3rd marriages
(2.7%). Twelve of these multiple marriages were to interracial partners. Thus,
the significance of the high prevalence rates of interracial, interfaith, and
interethnic relationships among persons with aberrant common sense remains to
be explored beyond being a mere caveat emptor. More than 70% of Proposita “A’s”
partners are interracial.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Proposita “D” was
considered to be strange, challenging, difficult, moody, emotionally labile,
worried well, anxiety-laden, frequently prone to errors and misunderstandings,
unreliable, and self-absorbed. In this study, Proposita “D” is one case control
for Proposita “A” insofar as her age is the same age as Proposita “A,” she and
Proposita “A” are second generation survivors of World War II, and Proposita
“D” has members of her family who were displaced by World War II. Proposita “D”
has a teenage, interfaith/interethnic son (Propositus “E”) who is approximately
five years younger than Proposita “C.” Propositus “E” is selected as a case
control for Proposita “C” particularly in view of findings on psychoviruses
(Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992; Smith, 2004c) and higher prevalence
rates associated with anxiety disorders in families. The significance of the
latter will become apparent later in this report.
[42] Both Propositi
“D” and “E” are cited in earlier studies (Smith, 1988; Smith, 2004b; Smith,
2004c; Smith, 2006a; Smith, 2006b; Smith, 2007a; Smith, 2007b). Proposita “D”
is Jewish and, as noted, a second generation survivor of World War II. Her
father’s family is Sephardic Jewish, although he was born in Poland and
migrated to Canada before the Holocaust. Her mother is Ashkenazi Jewish who,
along with her (mother’s) sister, was hidden by French Catholic families on
farms in France. They were raised as Catholics. Propositi “A” is a non-Jewish
second generation World War II survivor, and Propositi “Z” and “Y” are first
generation World War II survivors whose religious heritages are unknown.
[43] By any description,
Propositi “C” and “E” would be regarded as non-autistic savants insofar as each
excels in some personal passion (that is, Proposita “C” is a child actress of
considerable acclaim, and Propositus “E” is an expert on Civil War history).
Propositus “E” alleges pass-life experiences and past-life regressions, though
this has never been assessed.
[44] Proposita “F”
is a second case control for Propositi “A” and “D.” She is a divorced
Japanese-American who lived her formative years in internment centers in
California (that is, from months shortly after her birth until the closure of
the internment centers). Thus, Proposita “F” may provide another perspective on
the impact and consequences of World War II on the development of common sense
and it aberrations.
[45] Of all
propositi, only Proposita “D” actively discusses World War II – a common
finding in Jewish Holocaust survivors and their offspring. Germans and
Japanese-American survivors of World War II and their offspring are less likely
to openly discuss war-time experiences.23 Indeed, it is somewhat
difficult to document Trümmerfrauen (that is, “rubble women”)24
activities in Germany, even though they played a significant role in the
reconstruction of select regions in Germany (because of the scarcity of males
due to deaths and infirmity caused by World War II). The
23 Different groups respond
differently to war and trauma. Armenians are increasingly vocal about their
1915 experiences. Cambodians are relatively mute regarding the “killing fields”
and displacements. Chinese only recently have begun to discuss the Cultural
Revolution. Thus, an analysis of rhetoric (including prose, poetry, art, film
and music) and divergences in common sense may have value if divergences in
common sense in responses to war and trauma are affirmed. 24 Because
their activities generally were not organized or coordinated, Trümmerfrauen could
be extremely important in one’s quest to understand the evolution and
development of common sense and aberrant common sense in post-World War II
Germans. However, there is no evidence that Propositi “A,” “B” and “C” are
progeny of Trümmerfrauen.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
investigator knows
relatively little about Propositi “Y” and “Z,” however they may be significant
in future studies because they are first generation survivors.
[46] Of more than
50 adult propositi in a database of aberrant common sense (Smith, 1987; Smith,
1992; Smith, 2004c), there is very little evidence of significant religious or
spiritual practices among the propositi. None of the in situ propositi
revealed any spiritual practice, even though Propositi “A,” “B” and “C” are
Roman Catholic. Propositi “A” refused to pray in two situations where prayer
may have been appropriate or indicated. This general observation could provide
an opportunity for further investigation, particularly in the context of
resistance, intransigence and “my way or the highway” responses in most persons
lacking common sense. Faith in higher powers, in contrast to hope from the
occult, lies at the core of this concern.
[47]
Proposita “A” is the master tenant in a house owned by the investigator, as is
her daughter
Proposita “C” A third unrelated young adult male (Propositus “W”) also resides
in the household.
Proposita “B” is the first-born child of Proposita “A,” and is a university
student living
approximately one hour away from her mother and sister.
[48]
As reported earlier, there were advance plans for a two month scholarly
retreat. These plans
were negotiated with Proposita “A” at least six months prior to the planned
visit. Despite this
agreement, Proposita “A” may have changed her mind, although this was never
communicated
to the investigator.25 Because of egregious, profound and ongoing
instances of extreme passive-
aggression in Proposita “A” upon his arrival in Germany, the focus of this
research was changed
to identify and understand possible reasons underlying those aberrant
behaviors.
[49]
The diagnosis of aberrant common sense in Proposita “A” was made on 20 May 2007
after
more than one and a half months of observations,26 visits with
relatives, family, friends and other
acquaintances, and, direct observations of breakages, mistakes,
misunderstanding, and
numerous inappropriate actions. The thoroughness of this investigation was
deemed essential
because Proposita “A” revealed no obvious aberrancies in common sense in the
past, and
because of the sanctity of the business (that is, landlord – tenant)
relationship.
[50]
The concept of passive-aggression was unknown to any propositi in this study.
Despite this,
Proposita “C” and her father (Propositus “X,” the ex-spouse of Proposita “A”)
are profoundly
passive-aggressive.27 This was observed in March 2005, during visits
in 2006, and also was
25 This is just one example of
Proposita “A’s” extreme passive-aggression. 26 The chaos at the
beginning of the in situ period was palpable. It then was necessary to
“rule out” borderline personality disorder, confabulations, severe
passive-aggression without other co-morbid disorders, etc. 27 This
finding raised two vexing issues. The first vexing issue concerns the earliest
manifestations of passive-aggression in Propositi “A,” “C” and “X.” This was
important in disambiguating possible vectorial actions of psychoviruses – ‘from
whom to whom’. The second vexing issue was whether aberrant common sense in
Propositi “A” and “X” may have been transferred to each other. After in situ
encounters with Proposita “A’s” mother, siblings and childhood friends, it was
apparent that Proposita “A” lacked common sense prior to meeting Propositus
“X.”
reported by
Proposita “A” in numerous telephone calls and on various occasions. The
father-daughter passive-aggression was so profound and frequent that it often
produced stress and anxiety in Proposita “A.” During the April-May retreat
period, Proposita “B” also cited evidence of her father’s profound
passive-aggression. Direct and indirect evidence, as well as testimony from
friends, confirmed that Propositus “X” lacks common sense.28
[51] During the
retreat period, aberrant common sense associated with Proposita “A,” and
secondarily with daughter Proposita “C,” contributed to chaos,
misunderstandings, property destruction, harmful and destructive personal and
social relations, and faltering landlord-tenant relationships. The purpose of
the present in situ phenomenological analysis is to document and
understand aberrations in common sense during the two month period. A goal is
to identify processes associated with aberrations in common sense, and reasons
for their destructive consequences. More important, because of ongoing interest
in the evolution and development of common sense in generations of Germans and
Holocaust survivors in post-World War II Germany, it was deemed important to
understand the etiology of aberrant common sense in situ. After all, it
also is important to understand and possibly rule-out any relationship of the
immediate instances of aberrant common sense to the broader issue of
divergences in common sense arising from World War II. This is the reason
Propositi “D,” “E” and “F” are incorporated in the study as case controls.
Can persons
with aberrant common sense shed light on “healthy” or “balanced” persons’ (that
is, common) sense – gesundermenschenverstand?
[52] Quite often
diseases are used to elucidate and explicate normal processes. For example,
research on slow viruses which contribute to dementia in brain and the immune
system led to a new model of evolution and long-term memories (Smith, 1979).
That same model accurately anticipated HIV/AIDS (Smith, 1983; Smith, 1984) and
more than 90 epigenetic diseases associated with gamma herpesviruses and
adenoviruses (Smith, 2003a). Linus Pauling’s groundbreaking research on sickle
cell anemia led to an understanding of the molecular basis for genetic diseases
(Pauling et al., 1949). Huntington’s disease is likely to shed light on
boundaries between proteomic and non-proteomic regions of genomes, as well as
relationships among cognitive and sensory-motor components. Hence, an ambitious
goal in the present research on aberrant common sense is to provide useful
clues to reality, consciousness and formation of beliefs – in addition to a
possible biological basis for common sense.
[53] Ponder these
questions: how can one diagnose aberrant common sense? What distinguishes
common sense from aberrant common sense? What nomenclature should be used to
describe persons who lack common sense? Is “aberrant” an appropriate term to
describe one having no common sense? Can psychiatry, clinical psychology and
other “helping” professions develop an awareness of, appreciation for, and therapeutic
approaches to “transmissible negativism,” “aberrant common sense,” “unknowing
neediness” and “worried wellness”? Why have other scholars failed to recognize
diseases of common sense, unknowing neediness, and worried
This does not rule
out Propositus “X” lacking common sense early in his childhood. Indeed,
indirect and second-
hand information support this proposition.
28 Passive-aggression and aberrant common sense represent different
psychological entities. Earlier studies on
“transmissible negativism” led to a hypothesis that ‘transmissible negativism’
psychoviruses could contribute to
aberrant common sense, particularly in young children between birth and
approximately age 6 (Smith, 1987; Smith,
1988; Smith, 1992; Smith, 2004c). However, not all persons with aberrant common
sense are negative.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
wellness? Insofar
as persons who do not have common sense generally will not self-refer
themselves for therapy and counseling, how can chaos and other harmful
behaviors be circumvented in this unknowing needy subpopulation? Finally, why
is there an absence of any concept of common sense or aberrant common sense in
textbooks on psychiatry, clinical psychology and other helping professions –
worldwide? Have our heads been stuck in the sand during the past century?
[54] Two years ago,
Smith (2005b) announced a tripartite model of evolution at the 3rd
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and
Psychotherapies. A visual aid was used to depict that only 1.2% of the
human genome comprises the “proteome” – the protein encoding region of the
genome. That is, only 1.2% of the DNA in cells can explain our genes. Those genes
and proteins are the principal “stuff” in Darwinian evolution. For purposes of
simplification, one might call this nature. A large fraction (say, 75%)
of the remaining ~98% of the genome sometimes is referred to as “junk” DNA, and
cannot be explained by Darwinian principles. It now is proposed that a small
fraction of this allegedly “junk DNA” is used to encode “common sense” – beginning
at birth and extending to one becoming roughly six years old (cf. Fulghum,
1986/2004). An empirical issue then becomes whether the encoding schema for
common sense are consonant or “common” in any ways for animals in a cohort. If
so, this could provide powerful and compelling evidence that herd behavior may
have a common biological basis. The general proposal is that nurturance
and other forms of long-term memories largely are encoded in non-proteomic
portions of the genome. Because this presentation focuses on persons who do not
have common sense, a general theory of common sense will be discussed and amply
documented elsewhere (Smith, 2004a; Smith, 2004b; Smith, 2004c; Smith, 2007a;
Smith, 2007b; Smith, 2007c; Smith, 2007d). Suffice it to say, aberrant common
sense may be encoded somewhere in non-proteomic regions, but this no longer is
the primary concern.
[55] This report is
about people who do not have common sense. Smith’s earliest observations of the
phenomenon of aberrant common sense occurred in 1985 (Smith, 1986; Smith,
1988). Nine children in a Sunnyvale, California Elementary School Mathematics
Lab revealed types of mistakes in mathematics that simply could not be
explained using any form of error analyses. Their responses were outliers by
virtually every assessment of errors. More will be said about those 1985
experiences near the end of this report on in situ phenomenological
analyses and experiences of the situation in Germany earlier this year.
[56] In earlier
reports (Smith, 2004c; Smith, 2006b; Smith, 2007a; Smith, 2007b; Smith, 2007c;
Smith, 2007d), common sense was defined as “core ‘nurturance’ in herds.”
Although this definition is vague, its intent was to underscore the uniqueness
of common sense in herd behaviors. Herds may include spiritual, ethnic,
cultural, sects, professional, or other groupings of living entities. In plain
language, the definition of common sense was meant to represent core
“nurturing” experiences possessed by most members of a herd. Nurturance
derives from parents, friends, community and other herd experiences – including
environmental influences. This is an embodiment of Marian Wright Edelman’s
reference to an African proverb that “it takes a village to raise a child.”
Aberrant common sense generally refers to outliers – both as individuals in the
herd and as thinking and problem-solving behaviors not shared by most members
of the herd. The village metaphor fails. Persons with aberrant common sense do
not seem to have acquired village or herd teachings / values. The vagueness in
the earlier definitions was meant to underscore uniqueness, variability and
value-laden aspects in taught, learned and nurtured experiences.
[57]
In the end, the present in situ phenomenological findings reveal that
neither of these definitions
– for common sense
or aberrant common sense – is adequate or sufficient to capture the salient
mental features central to logic and information processing in common sense. On
the surface,
nurturance should be both necessary and sufficient to enculturate all of the
needed logical skills
for survival and other practical decision-making. This in situ
phenomenological study reveals
otherwise. It underscores a need for a finer grained analysis of thinking,
cognition and
mentation. For example, earlier studies revealed the importance of mathematics
problem solving
and reading skills in commonsense and aberrant commonsense behaviors (Smith,
1987; Smith,
1988; Smith, 1992). Those studies also highlighted unusual error processes
associated
mathematics and reading in persons who lack common sense. For this study,
selected
mathematics, problem solving and deep reading skills are deficient in adult
Propositi “A,” “D,”
“F,” and younger propositi “C” and “E.” Their deficiencies teach that logistic
reasoning and
anticipatory skills are critically important in common sense, whereas the
absence of logistic
reasoning and anticipatory skills may be associated with frustration,
humiliation and anxiety
disorders. This may be a reason why Proposita “D” graduated cum laude from
University of
California, Berkeley and possess a law degree, yet still lack common sense.
Propositi “A,” “C,”
“D” and “F” are high-functioning, non-autistic individuals except when
confronted with extreme
tasks and/or critical thinking challenges. Then they become “unglued” and “come
apart.”
[58]
Perhaps more important, the scope of breakages, errors, mistakes,
misunderstandings and
misinterpretations are quite prevalent and prominent in persons lacking common
sense. Those
error processes often produce anxiety and generalized anxiety disorders over
extended periods
of time. Estimates of prevalence rates for anxiety disorders range from 3% -
17% in the general
population (Ries, 1996; Gater et al., 1998). Insofar as many of the propositi
in this study live
near Mainz, Germany and at a greater distance from Berlin, the prevalence rates
for generalized
anxiety disorders in Mainz are particularly intriguing (cf. Table 3 in Gater et
al., 1998). Yet,
because little is written about common sense, aberrant common sense and anxiety
disorders, it
is not known whether aberrant common sense is a major component of this
fraction.
[59]
In the present study, Proposita “A” is medicated for an anxiety disorder. She
revealed this to the
investigator and only one other person. None her children, siblings or other
family members
were informed of her medication. Proposita “D” resolutely refuses any medical
attention for her
frequent anxiety attacks – even upon recommendations of her primary care and
other
physicians. Proposita “C” is unmedicated, although she has consulted a clinical
psychotherapist
upon the recommendation of her primary care physician. Neither Propositi “A”
nor “C” is willing
to consider long-term psychotherapy (such as cognitive-behavioral therapy),
even with
substantial positive incentives and inducements.
[60]
Although many features characterize all propositi in this study (see Table 2),
two features of
aberrant common sense define all propositi in this study and propositi in the
larger database of
propositi with aberrant common sense. First, all propositi show limitations in
overall scope and
purview for broad issues (that is, they never are able to see the “big
picture”). Second, each of
the propositi
relies almost exclusively on her or his thinking – “it’s my way or the
highway.” Regarding the latter, there virtually is no evidence of “commonality”
in their common sense. Those personal limitations also may be sources of stress
and anxiety, especially when their reasoning skills fail them.
[61] Please view
the video clip of predator-prey behavior (involving water buffalo, lions, and a
crocodile/alligator) in South Africa’s Kruger National Park (Schlosberg and
Budzinski, 2004; see <www.youtube.com/watch?v=LU8DDYz68kM>).29 The
videoclip shows a herd of water buffalos meandering in one direction, with a
pride of predator lions lying in wait at a distal point in that direction. The
lions eventually attack the water buffaloes, ultimately mauling one calf. Other
(more senior) water buffalo return to the herd and, by some unknown means,
summon its support to defend themselves, the calf being mauled, and their
“space” (Schlosberg and Budzinski, 2004). This videoclip poses several
questions. Does water buffalo behavior involve “common sense”? By what means
did the returning water buffaloes communicate the need for a ‘common sense’
response to the lion attack? How and when did these behaviors develop and/or
evolve? Is there any “common” encoding of “common sense” in non-proteomic
regions of the water buffalo genome (in their brains)? Through this simple
example, one may see that common sense may not be limited to humans. One also
may appreciate why the term “herd” (instead of “group” or some similar word)
has value for indicating common or collective behavior. This example also
provides theoretical support for studies of divergences in common sense arising
from war and trauma. Ultimately, if and when non-invasive imaging of G*C :: A*T
ratios in regions of brain can be recorded, one may seek evidence of
divergences in common sense in non-proteomic portions of the genome after
traumatic situations similar to the Battle at Kruger.
[62] The Battle of
Kruger video clip has value for another reason. If the mauled calf survived,
ponder how being traumatized by the lions and crocodile will affect its
post-traumatic common sense. How might the calf’s common sense diverge from
other members of the herd? What will be the long-term developmental and
evolutionary consequences of that traumatic experience for the specific calf
and its offspring? In other words, this remarkable video clip encapsulates and
embodies many, if not most, of the elements this investigator initially planned
to explicate for this conference – though with the human focus being the
developmental and evolutionary consequences of post-World War II German and
Holocaust survivors, and their offspring.
[63] Because this
report focuses on people lacking common sense, it is appropriate to ask if
there are animal models of aberrant common sense. Crabs (that is, selected
crustaceans) seem to lack common sense, a phenomenon observed as a child on
family fishing expeditions. Crabs often were caught on the ocean side in
quantities which would fill “bushel” baskets. The crabs never seemed to exhibit
cooperative behaviors among themselves, always pursuing their personal pathways
– even if it involved stepping all over there basket mates. One also sees this
type of behavior in fish and crustacean holding tanks in Chinese markets and
restaurants
29 The Battle at Kruger
– a battle over a water buffalo calf between a pride of lions, a herd of
buffalo, and a crocodile
– was witnessed by
Jason Schlosberg and David Budzinski in September 2004 (Schlosberg and
Budzinski, 2004) at a watering hole near Pretoriuskop Camp, Mpumalanga in South
Africa's Kruger National Park. Copies of this videoclip can be obtained from
NegativeSpace at <http://www.negativespace.com/battle.htm>.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
worldwide.
Even though cooperation is not synonymous with common sense, an extremely
important hallmark of aberrant common sense is a person’s “my way or the
highway” or “go it alone” belief construct. This is the basis for the statement
”my way or the highway” meaning that ‘I am always correct and refuse to
consider other views or thoughts’.
[64] As
mentioned earlier, in 1985 nine elementary school students in grades three
through grade six were observed to have aberrant common sense. The elementary
school was in Sunnyvale, CA
– in the
heart of “Silicon Valley.” Aberrant common sense was diagnosed based on
student’s answers to selected mathematics problems. In mathematics, one
generally has one or more correct answers, and other possible error responses.
In most instances, correct and error responses are rather narrowly
circumscribed. The 9 students’ responses were outliers to reasonable
circumscribed error responses. Their responses had a quality of being unbelievable
and outrageous. In a fortuitous conversation with the school librarian (where
the math laboratory was being held during non-library hours), the school
librarian mentioned that those same 9 students had reading difficulties. They
had difficulties with reading prose, and invented stories from cartoons. She
cited examples from a Japanese Elementary Morality Education textbook to
illustrate her point (Umiuchi, 1967; Exhibit 4).Suffice it to say, another
hallmark of aberrant common sense is that math and reading skills often are
limited.
[65] The
1985 experience included another surprising observation. When parents were
contacted regarding their child’s performance (and, obviously without using
the term “not having any common sense”), those parents were observed to be
uniformly negative. The term psychovirus then was coined to explain the
presumptive transmissible and infectious psychological process contributing to
aberrant common sense (Smith, 1986; Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992).
Experiences in Germany were similar. Persons who lack common sense may have
been exposed to profound negativity and/or be profoundly negative themselves.
[66] The
in situ study revealed the profound importance of misunderstanding
and misinterpretation in aberrant common sense. The terms
misunderstanding and misinterpretation are unrelated to one’s native (first)
language. In other words, the fact that German was the spoken language, and the
investigator’s first language is English, is not relevant.30 Rather, propositi
with aberrant common sense often state or do things which are outrageously
preposterous and/or unbelievably thoughtless.31,32 The term “chaos” is too kind
a term to describe consequences of
30
Proposita A” is reasonably fluent in the English language and Propositi “B” and
“C” are fluent in English and are American citizens. 31 The term ‘thoughtless
is a relatively neutral term used in lieu of ‘stupid’ and ‘stupidity’ – even
though the latter are more accurate in some instances. The latter are harsh and
likely to be too “charged.” The challenge was to find terms which do not
imply that one innately is incompetent, unable, or has an inability or mental
deficiency. Persons with aberrant common sense generally are neither dull,
incompetent, unable, nor mentally deficient. Rather, their momentary responses
and mental lapses in challenging situations reveal a quality of
thoughtlessness, lacking judgment and/or being unaware, unthinking and
transfixed. These transfixed behaviors may lead to psychopathology – including
passive-aggression, cynicism, frustration, stress, anxiety, and resistance,
intransigence and “my way or the highway” responses. Although somewhat
counterintuitive, the insensitivities and lack of judgment and awareness may be
sources of underlying sociopathy. More important, ‘believable’ is used to
describe certain types of circumscribable mistakes and errors (as in
mathematics; cf. Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988; Smith, 2004c). ‘Unbelievable’ is
used to describe outliers which challenge one’s credulity. In aberrant common
sense, errors, mistakes, misunderstandings, and breakages often are of the
unbelievable sort.
Copyright
© 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
these
phenomena. Indeed, any underlying chaos serves to underscore the challenges
regarding whether the classification of aberrant common sense is a
psychopathological or a sociopathological disorder. It is both! Persons with
aberrant common sense are both psychopaths and sociopaths.33
[67]
In studies of aberrant common sense over many years, persons with aberrant
common sense
often are extremely passive-aggressive. Procrastination and tardiness often are
hallmarks of
passive-aggression. An extreme form of passive-aggression can involve
procrastination and
tardiness over many months. There is an “I will show you (that it will be my
way)” attitude unlike
in other milder forms of passive-aggression. Passive-aggression is manifest in
other ways.
Cynicism and internecine (that is, “back-stabbing) behaviors are rampant – and
especially in
responses to negativity. Wit and humor often are relentlessly stinging and
unsparing. Proposita
“F” uses the term “zingers” to describe these stinging barbs and wit.
[68]
Many of the in situ findings for the German and other case control
propositi are summarized in
Table 2. The following comments provide additional vignettes about German
propositi which
could not be reported in Table 2, and especially during the April-May retreat.
32 The term
‘thoughtless’ often does not capture underlying ‘stupidity’. Despite her
healthcare profession background, Proposita “A” on several occasions used a
table knife to obtain and spread jelly and/or quark on bread, and then licked
the knife before sticking it back into the jelly jar. Somewhat later, she was
surprised that the jam was moldy. When this behavior was pointed out to
Proposita “A” immediately after it occurred, she vehemently denied her
behavior. Another Swedish propositus in the larger aberrant common sense
database also seeded bacteria in jellies using the same mechanism (Smith, 1992;
Smith, 2004c). Proposita “C” often would hoard bread, jellies and other
products passive-aggressively, and then express surprise when products became
moldy and/or rancid. 33 ‘Sociopaths’ are antisocial-personality disorders or
dissocial personality disorders (ICD-9 301.7; ICD-10 F60.2).
Several propositi (with aberrant common sense show) show a disregard for social
norms, are impulsive and nonstrategic thinkers, unwittingly cause harm to
themselves and others, and, disregard others’ feelings and rights. Insofar as
Germans are highly law-abiding persons, disregard for rights in the present
context refers to non-codified rights and laws though which are social norms.
Proposita “C” did not understand why it was inappropriate to ‘crash’ a
neighbor’s dinner party, and Proposita “A” justified her daughter’s behavior on
the grounds that the neighbor had enough food to accommodate extra guests. This
example is particularly salient insofar as cultural norms in Germany may differ
from cultural norms in Armenia or Bulgaria – two cultures in which dinner
guests may be openly embraced. 34 It is for this reason that negativism and
aberrant common sense was present in Proposita “A” prior to any mitigating
circumstances cited earlier.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
35 Reference to sex addiction
is used advisedly. This investigator respects and fully appreciates the needs
and challenges single and divorced persons face when pursuing fulfilling loving
and sexual relationships. In the context of aberrant common sense, the sole
concern is about high risk behaviors with no apparent concern for risks to others
(see Exhibit 2). 36 Aberrant common sense does not imply that all
decision-making and problem-solving behaviors are defective. The terms
‘kinetic’ and ‘potential’ are meant to underscore this important distinction.
This also is a reason why persons who lack common sense can be highly
functional and successful. A challenge in future research will be to assess
which decision-making and problem-solving skills are defective, and, if
possible, to teach or properly nurture acceptable skills and behaviors.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
a.
o
Aberrant sensitivities (Proposita “A”)
b.
o
Impractical concerns and knowledge (Proposita “A”)
c.
o
Inappropriate body language (Proposita “A”) [Note: This is new in the
investigator’s research on common sense.]
d.
o
Irrational fears (Proposita “A” and “C”)
e.
o
Discernment (Propositi “A” and “C”)
f.
o
Lack of mindfulness (Propositi “A” and “C”)
g.
o
Disorders in groundedness and embodiment (Proposita “A”)
37 In today's world, there are
many products which are replaced rather than being repaired, in part, because
costs for repairs are greater than the price for replacing the product. At the
same time, this could represent an example of divergent common sense from the
common sense of earlier (say, one’s grandparents’) generations. In
contradistinction, the types of breakages without repairs associated with
persons with aberrant common sense represent extreme and outlying behaviors.38
Mollie Spring, personal communication
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
<www.sparknotes.com/psychology/abnormal/anxiety/summary.html>). Therefore it is not
surprising that Propositi “A” and “D” are resistant to psychotherapy.39
[69] As noted
earlier, it immediately was apparent in March 2005 that Proposita “C” lacked
common sense. At that time, there were no indications that Propositi “A” or “B”
lacked common sense. Indeed, observations of Propositi “A,” “B” and “C”
provided the initial breakthrough leading to the tripartite model of evolution.
Proposita “B” was a healthy, happy, joyful and wanted child – particularly
during pregnancy and the first six years of her life. When Proposita “A” became
pregnant with Proposita “C,” tensions and anxieties immediately developed
between Propositi “A” and “X.” During her formative years, Proposita “C” was
needy, constantly clamoring for parental affection, manipulative, and often
sleeping with her parents even beyond 7 years old.40
[70] It is not
surprising that Proposita “A” constantly demonstrates unconditional love and
sacrifice for Propositi “B” and “C.” This represents “motherly love,” even
though Proposita “C” continually
39 Despite her
resistance to psychotherapy, Proposita “A” is medicated for anxiety attacks
allegedly brought on by her work environment. This study reveals an additional
possibility that aberrant common sense may have contributed to her anxiety.40
Succinctly, Proposita “B” was a planned and desired pregnancy. Proposita “C”
was an unplanned pregnancy, and a source of considerable (albeit somewhat
latent) marital strife and tension between Propositi “A” and “X.” Insofar as
the tripartite theory distinguishes between pregnancy (that is, in utero
or intrauterine) and after-birth experiences
– in both the
mother and her offspring - this finding could have significance in terms of
disambiguating in utero
contributions and
nurturance after childbirth.
Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
uses extreme
passive-aggression to manipulate her mother. When Proposita “A” was diagnosed
with aberrant common sense, a seeming contradiction (between unconditional love
and sacrifice for her daughters, on the one hand, and extremely poor parenting,
on the other hand) was resolved. Unconditional love and parental nurturance was
beneficial for Proposita “B,” whereas unconditional love and poor nurturance
may have been detrimental for Proposita “C.”
[71] Regardless of
the accuracy of any underlying theoretical characterization of the family
dynamics, findings in this study provide additional support for psychoviruses
in the etiology of some aberrations in common sense. These findings also lend
support to observations of that anxiety disorders often are reported in
families. Whether psychoviruses will be implicated in these familial findings
remains to be explicated. The intersection between stress-induced consequences
of EBV infections (Smith, 2003a), one the one hand, and psychovirus
consequences, on the other hand, also should be a matter for further study.
Perhaps most important, findings in this in situ study underscore the
importance of reasoning, problem-solving and critical thinking skills in common
sense and aberrant common sense.
[72]
Many medical and psychiatric disorders accompany aberrant common sense. As
noted earlier,
general anxiety disorder (ICD-10 – F41.1; ICD-9 – 300.02) perhaps
is the most common of
these disorders, particularly in adults. It often is characterized by
uncontrollable and excessive
worry about work, health, appearance, money, family problems, and petty issues.
Irrational
worries and misunderstandings also are common findings. Physical symptoms
include rapid
heart beat, sweating, difficulties breathing, feeling tense, muscle tensions,
muscle aches,
trembling, irritability, fatigue and headaches (cf. Hoehn-Saric et al, 2004).
Stress-induced
Epstein-Barr virus (EBV)-related epigenetic disorders (Smith, 2003a) also are
common
presenting symptoms. Propositi “A” and “C” regularly report each and all of
these psychological
and physical findings. Although these findings can be anticipated in persons
lacking common
sense, it remains unclear if cognitive-behavioral therapies or other
psychotherapeutic
approaches can beneficially resolve any aberrations in common sense.
[73]
Findings in this study raise a more important issue. To what extent are
aberrations in common
sense substantial components in anxiety disorders, and, vice versa, to what
extents do anxiety
disorders presage and anticipate aberrations in common sense?
[74] Persons who
lack common sense are unlikely to seek any forms of psychotherapy (cf. <http://www.sparknotes.com/psychology/abnormal/anxiety/summary.html>). A “friends can be
good medicine” approach was used because Proposita “A” is extremely resistant
to seeking psychotherapy. This particular approach was devised by the
California State Department of Mental Health in the 1980s (Hunter and
Lloyd-Kolkin, 1883; Hersey et al., 1984; Taylor et al., 2004; cf. Exhibit 3).
Three different posters were framed and presented to Proposita “A.” The
overarching motivation and perspective is captured in a Kenneth Patchen
picture-poem; to wit, “Caring is the only daring. Oh you better believe it.”
After a three month period using these approaches, Proposita A shows no
willingness to pursue any form of professional therapy – despite optimism, and
substantial inducements and incentives. This study teaches that conventional
and unconventional approaches to care-giving may not resolve fundamental
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
challenges of convincing persons with aberrant
common sense to seek psychotherapy. The rampant psychopathic and sociopathic
behaviors in Propositi “A” and “C” continue unabated.
Some ‘Meta’-Transpersonal Issues and Perspectives
[75] A number of
presentations at the 4th International Conference on Humanistic and
Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies intersect with the present
study. For example, inclusion of this presentation in a round-table forum on
“Problems with Legal Determination of Psychosis” highlighted a need for
clinicians and transpersonalists to appreciate interdisciplinary issues in law,
psychiatry, psychology and the judiciary. Another presentation (Zhang, 2007)
raised a question of whether transmissible negativism, poor nurturance, and/or
aberrant common sense may be an underlying factor in a young man’s
psychological and art therapy challenges. Professor Arons’ presentation of
Zimbardo’s research on heroism (Franco et al., 2007; Zimbardo, 2006) posed
issues of heroism versus common sense. This section considers some of those
intersecting issues and how they serve to germinate newer and more generalized
perspectives – meta-perspectives. Also considered in this section are
lessons learned from the particular in situ phenomenological study –
much alike the last panel in the second Japanese SHOGAKU DŌU TŌKU
cartoon in Exhibit 4 (see Footnote 18)? A third challenge is to reflect on
parsimony and continuity, particularly in the context of reports at the 3rd
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and
Psychotherapies. The present study is a parsimonious extension to earlier
discussions on slow viruses, Darwinism and evolution (Smith, 2005b).
[76] Overall, these
concerns represent meta-transpersonal perspectives. To avoid being any
more diffuse, discursive or detracting from the principal in situ
findings in this study, this section is presented in outline form rather than
as a narrative. A thorough explication of these issues will appear in Smith (in
preparation).
[77]
a.
o
although predicting futures may have dubious reliability and validity, logistic
reasoning (Smith, 1983) may lead to better- or well-circumscribed possibilities
(Smith, 1979; also see the “Did you know” videoclip <http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=xHWTLA8WecI>)
b.
o
trauma of technological futures – impact of $100 computer on common sense in
third world countries
c.
o
PTSD and other consequences of war and trauma (cf. Milliken, Auchterlonie and
Hoge, 2007)
[78]
a.
o
Revised test theory based on peer influences on individual differences
b.
o
Peer review when there are no peers (Horrobin, 1882)
[79]
a.
o
Genomics – proteomics versus non-proteomics
b.
o
Changing G*C::A*T ratios in selected regions in brain
c.
o
Non-invasive imaging of changing DNA in brain
d.
o
fMRI studies
e.
o
Disambiguation and refinement of the autism spectrum, temporary autism
(Gladwell, 2005), common sense, aberrant common sense
a.
o
Transmissible negativism and national caveats emptor (Smith, 1987; Smith, 1988;
Smith, 1992)
b.
o
The biogenesis, development and evolution of terrorism (Smith, 2002)
c.
o
Common sense as a concrete marker of peace
d.
o
Common sense and terrorism – does terrorism beget terrorism, and can peace
beget peace
1.
o
Peace – terrorism – forgiveness – common sense (Regarding terrorism and forgiveness,
see stories of Michael Berg and Peter Katz on Canadian Broadcast
2.
Company
“As It Happens” radio new clips for 8 June 2006 <http://www.cbc.ca/world/story/2006/06/08/berg-father.html> and 13 November 2007
<http://www.cbc.ca/radioshows/AS_IT_HAPPENS/20071113.shtml>)
e.
o
“Truth and reconciliation” as an adjunct to divergences in common sense
a.
o
The economics of common sense and common knowledge (for example, the cost of
HIV/AIDS associated with failures in common sense and/or limitations in common
knowledge)
b.
o
The economics of knowing when and how to stop (for example, addictions,
gambling, attempts at making vaccines against HIV, experiments involving select
approaches to gene therapies [Smith, 2007e (unpublished)], or to shift
paradigms [Smith, 2007a; Smith, 2007b; Smith, 2007c; Smith, in preparation];
cf. chapters 29, 30, 32, and 44 in Lao Tzu’s Tao Te Ching [Yi Wu, 1989;
Keith H. Seddon, date after 1989])
a.
o
Newly articulated transpersonal and peer-based test theories
b.
o
“Psychovaccines” against psychoviruses
a.
o
Biofeedback
b.
o
fMRI studies,
c.
o
valid and reliable, non-invasive technologies for assessing psychological
illness, health, wellness and holistic embodiment (for example, assessing syntropy
[Smith, 2007] using non-invasive determinations of Guanine*Cytosine ::
Adenine*Thymine DNA base-pair ratios [Smith, 2007a; Smith, 2007b; Smith,
2007c])
a.
o
The example of the spread of HIV/AIDS and other sexually transmitted diseases
(cf. Moss et al., 2007) in university settings is discussed above, and
demonstrates the importance for transpersonal psychology to have a pulse on
social and epidemiologic issues
b.
o
Increasing and improving common knowledge and common sense
c.
o
Epidemiological studies of common and uncommon findings – including common
sense, aberrant common sense, common pathogens in HIV/AIDS, uncommon and
nascent pathogens in HIV/AIDS
[80]
Common Sense
in Science, Scientific ‘Common Sense’, and Aberrations in Scientific ‘Common
Sense’
[81]
Looking Back
– what have we learned?
[82]
a.
o
Definitions – recasting definition in terms of descriptive-structuralism,
heuristic-functionalism, and logistic reasoning (cf. Ennis, 1984)
b.
o
Purpose versus function – as in changes in DNA in brain
c.
o
Common sense
d.
o
Aberrant common sense
e.
o
Impractical, thoughtless and “stupid” behaviors
a.
o
Passive-aggression given aberrant common sense, and aberrant common sense given
passive-aggression
b.
o
{Pr[ pa | acs ] = Pr[ pa and acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
c.
o
{Pr[ pa | acs ] = Pr[ pa ∩ acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
d.
o
{Pr[ pa | acs ] = (Pr[ acs | pa ] x Pr[ pa ]) / ((Pr[ acs | pa ] x Pr [ pa ]) +
(Pr [ acs | not pa ] x Pr[ not pa ]))}
e.
o
{Pr[ pa | acs ] = (Pr[ acs | pa ] x Pr[ pa ]) / ((Pr[ acs | pa ] x Pr [ pa ]) +
(Pr [ acs | ¬pa ] x Pr[ ¬pa ]))}
f.
o
Anxiety given aberrant common sense, and aberrant common sense given anxiety
g.
o
{Pr[ an | acs ] = Pr[ an and acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
h.
o
{Pr[ an | acs ] = Pr[ an ∩ acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
i.
o
{Pr[ an | acs ] = (Pr[ acs | an ] x Pr[ an ]) / ((Pr[ acs | an ] x Pr [ an ]) +
(Pr [ acs | not an ] x Pr[ not an ]))}
j.
o
{Pr[ an | acs] = (Pr[ acs | an] x Pr[ an ]) / ((Pr[ acs | an ] x Pr [ an ]) +
(Pr [ acs | ¬an ] x Pr[ ¬an ]))}
k.
o
‘Transmissible negativism given aberrant common sense, and aberrant common
sense given ‘transmissible negativism’
l.
o
{Pr[ tn | acs ] = Pr[ tn and acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
m.o {Pr[ tn | acs ] = Pr[ tn
∩ acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
n.
o
{Pr[ tn | acs ] = (Pr[ acs | tn ] x Pr[ tn ]) / ((Pr[ acs | tn ] x Pr [ tn ]) +
(Pr [ acs | not tn ] x Pr[ not tn ]))}
o.
o
{Pr[ tn | acs] = (Pr[ acs | tn ] x Pr[ tn ]) / ((Pr[ acs | tn ] xPr [ tn ]) +
(Pr [ acs | ¬tn ] x Pr[ ¬tn ]))}
p.
o
Race/culture in relationships given aberrant common sense, and aberrant common
sense given race/culture relationships
q.
o
{Pr[ rcr | acs ] = Pr[ rcr and acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
r.
o
{Pr[ rcr | acs ] = Pr[ rcr ∩ acs ] / Pr[ acs ]}
s.
o
{Pr[ rcr | acs ] = (Pr[ acs | rcr ] x Pr[ rcr ]) / ((Pr[ acs | rcr ] x Pr [ rcr
]) + (Pr [ acs | not rcr ] x Pr[ not rcr ]))}
t.
o
{Pr[ rcr | acs] = (Pr[ acs | rcr ] x Pr[ rcr ]) / ((Pr[ acs | rcr ] x Pr [ rcr
]) + (Pr [ acs | ¬rcr ] x Pr[ ¬rcr ]))}
a.
o
Insofar as terrorism may beget terrorism (cf. <http://www.cbc.ca/world/story/2006/06/08/berg-father.html>), one should not
overlook possibilities that peace may beget forgiveness and peace (cf. <http://www.cbc.ca/radioshows/AS_IT_HAPPENS/20071113.shtml>)
b.
o
Aberrant common sense and terrorism
[83] In ending this
report, participants at the 4th International Conference on
Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and Psychotherapies discussed and
debated eastern versus western versus Chinese psychology. Those discussions
serve to underscore a central tenet in this report. Transpersonalists and
others are overlooking fundamental opportunities to move humanity forward, not
merely as easterners, westerners or Chinese – especially in booming China.
Professor Arons cited another cogent example – that of a child running into a
street of oncoming traffic. His example was meant to explicate and elucidate a
point about heroism (Franco et al., 2007; Zimbardo, 2006). In the context of discussions
on common sense, it is common sense, and not heroism or courage, which
would propel a person to rescue the child. It is common sense which can propel
humanity forward – and serve as a concrete marker of peace. Again, this was
nicely illustrated in the YouTube “Battle at Kruger” videoclip when water
buffaloes took responsibilities for themselves and their kin-calf (see <http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=LU8DDYz68kM>).
Conclusions
[84] Earlier
studies of aberrant common sense identified selected features associated its
clinical presentations (Smith, 1988; Smith, 1992). This report represents the
first known study of aberrant common sense in situ. Neither in situ
nor phenomenological studies are new. For example, Robert Coles’ classic and
award-winning reports on race and trauma in children (initially inspired by a
young first grade black student surrounded by an angry mob; Coles, 1995)
represent in situ and phenomenological analyses (Coles, 1967; Coles,
1977; Coles, 1986; Coles, 1989; Coles, 1990; Coles, 2000; Coles, 2003). The
present in situ phenomenological approach to studying aberrant common
sense and unknowing neediness provided a unique opportunity to observe,
first-hand, processes underlying the extensive chaos, trauma,
dysfunctional family and social interactions. Significantly, this study also
highlights subtle costs (to the propositi and others) and other economic
considerations associated with this disorder. This approach also provided an
opportunity to document the transmissibility and infectiousness of occasional
underlying ‘psychovirus’ phenomena.
[85] Coles’ studies
serve to inform this research in another way. Although his observations about
race and trauma in children have never focused on aberrant common sense and
especially its putative relationship to peace, many of his studies of children
worldwide do lend support to our hypothesis that war and trauma may contribute
to divergences in common sense. Sadly and regrettably, because there are no
references to common sense or any forms of aberrant common sense cited in any
textbooks in psychiatry and other clinical and educational professions, the
potential significance and profoundness of this matter may have been misplaced
or overlooked.
[86] Three
approaches to scientific reasoning were discussed in a previous report (Smith,
1983) – especially in life sciences. These approaches are descriptive-structuralism
(DS), heuristic-functionalism (HF), and logistic reasoning (LR).
The present in situ phenomenological analyses of a mother–daughter
co-propositi, when combined with an ongoing study of a mother–son
coproposita–propositus, reveal the absolute centrality and importance in
understanding definitions and underlying processes (cf. Ennis, 1974).
Definitions may include descriptions and nomenclature, structures,
synchronic and diachronic issues, individual and group processes,
dynamics (that is, dynamic changes in both structures and processes), evolution
and more (Smith, in preparation). The latter includes external factors –
including economics, environment, social ecology, discrepancies in “reality,”
ethnomethodology, philosophy of science, and metaissues. In the present study,
processes and dynamics could only be discovered using the in
situ phenomenological approach – thus adding further support for HF and LR,
especially in life science research.
[87] In the end, a
definition of common sense is more elusive than ever! Core nurturance is a
necessary but not sufficient component in common sense. In situ findings
reveal that active (kinetic) processes also are necessary. Knowledge alone is
not sufficient (see Footnote 46). Although it is not reasonable to expect
error-free processes and performance, common sense processes are
well-circumscribed and concordant with herd behaviors. Aberrant common sense
involves outlier behavior beyond some herd- or culture-circumscribed norms.
Preliminary evidence suggests that psychoviruses, trauma and other situational
factors may contribute to the profoundness of any outliers – and divergences in
common sense.
[88] Insofar as
realities may be shaped by nurturance, psychoviruses, trauma and other factors,
the present report highlights the importance in distinguishing and
disambiguating experiences and realities. Consider the example of Helen Anna
Keller (Keller, 1961). She used her disabilities and experiential limitations
(due to her deafness and blindness) to remarkable advantages involving
realities consistent with her and others’ abilities. Even though she was never
evaluated for any aberrations in common sense, most scholars would agree that
her tenacity and successes are indicative of some measures of reality and
common sense – despite her disabilities. This report on aberrant common sense
reveals the need to appreciate sensory deficits (e.g., in Helen Keller) in
contrast to deficits in cognitive processing (such as “functional strokes”; see
below) in aberrant common sense.
[89] Although these
matters may not be intractable, findings in this study reveal extreme
difficulties in dealing with persons with aberrant common sense – especially
when those behaviors are florid and out-of-control. This research underscores
the need for psychiatry and other clinical
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
professions
(including education, psychoanalysis, psychotherapy and clinical social work)
to recognize and respond to aberrations in common sense. It also may be
important to set realistic objectives for persons who lack common sense. They
possibly will never be sufficiently healthy or well to develop “common” common
sense. In the particular situation in Germany, the term “functional stroke” was
used to make a comparison to a “structural stroke” in which a region of brain
is dead because of the loss of vascularization. Persons who lack common sense
effectively experience functional strokes insofar as they may not fully access
or use functional regions of brain. This hypothesis is testable using fMRI and
other downstream technologies (e.g., non-invasive imaging devices capable of
imaging changing G*C :: A*T ratios). The profound clinical question is
whether such “functional strokes” are amenable to treatment and/or change using
psychotherapy and/or psycho-pharmacotherapy interventions. Optimism must be the
order of the day in view of neuronal plasticity in brain.
[90] Although a
parent-child dyad is central to the present analyses of Propositi “A” and “C,”
other parent-child and intergenerational instances of aberrant common sense
also have been investigated (Smith, 1992; Smith, 2004c; Smith, 2007a). Those
studies lacked the invaluable in situ observations. The case report by
Zhang (2007) is suggestive of other issues involving common sense which only
became apparent during art therapy. Zhang’s client clearly exhibited neuroses,
although common sense findings also may be present.
[91] In general,
psychoviruses can be invoked to explain the transmission of aberrant common
sense in all of the cases studied to-date including the propositi in this
study. It is important to stress that no direct connection was established
between Propositi “A,” “B,” “C,” “Y” and “Z” and consequences of World War II.
This is a matter which was not treated lightly though. The discovery of four
cases of aberrant common sense within less than 100 square meters possibly
could be a statistical anomaly, or it could represent a significant statistical
finding. It is known that Trümmerfrauen (“debris women”) played significant
roles in the reconstruction of Germany immediately after World War II. Their
behaviors were not coordinated. Hence, there are sound theoretical reasons for
anticipating increased prevalence rates of aberrant common sense in post-World
War II Germany, yet the present results do not support any general
conclusions.41 In view of earlier remarks regarding common sense and peace,
this matter must not be misplaced or trivialized.
[92] The earlier
discussion cited issues of reality. This study further affirms a need for
increased attention to disambiguate aberrant beliefs, aberrant realities, and
aberrant common sense. What
41 There is no
evidence that Trümmerfrauen, trauma or other hardship experiences contributed
to Proposita “A’s” aberrant common sense. Available evidence suggests that
Proposita “A” had a middle-class upbringing near the German-Dutch border. Some
evidence (from her mother, siblings and childhood friends) reveals that her
parents were very diligent, industrious and successful innkeepers. During this
period in her parents’ career, her parents unwittingly may have
neglected Proposita “A” during her formative years and during the rapid
reconstruction and revitalization of West Germany in the 1950s. Not too
surprisingly, this unwitting parental neglect is quite similar to Proposita
“C’s” formative development when Proposita “A” and “X” were at odds. In other
words, aberrant common sense in Proposita “A” indirectly may have given rise to
parental neglect in Proposita “C.” Both findings possibly can be attributed to
the emergent economy in West Germany after World War II. Moreover,
Proposita “A’s” birth order placed her early development right at the time her
parents were aggressively developing their business interest. As the second of
six siblings, this also might explain Proposita “A’s” relationships with her
siblings. However, these matters require further investigation. At the very
least, these findings underscore the importance of consistency in common sense
and parental responsiveness to birth order and other fractious elements in
nurturance.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
is crystal clear is
that distorted reality is a prima facie characteristic of persons with
aberrant common sense.
[93] An example of
the economics of aberrant common sense was cited earlier. The Introduction
cited plans to complete an analysis of data associated with common sense in
Germans when contrasted to common sense in Holocaust survivors in the USA and
Holocaust survivors in Israel. The household strife, upheavals and dynamics
associated with aberrant common sense in the co-propositi presented real costs
and lost opportunities. Other aspects of economics of common sense and aberrant
commonsense may be even important. Just as gambling and other addictions
represent failures in knowing when or how to ‘stop’ clinically (see outline
of metatranspersonal perspectives), and there may be needs for “national
caveats emptor” (Smith, 1986; Smith, 1987; Smith, 1992), aberrant scientific
common sense also inflicts a huge economic toll. The unwarranted and avoidable
deaths of Jolee Mohr and Jesse Gelsinger are two cases in point (Smith, 2007e;
cf. Smith, 1983; Smith, 1984; Smith, 2001). Recent failures in an HIV vaccine
trail represent another example (Timberg, 2007a; Timberg, 2007b; Timberg,
2007c). Furthermore, inappropriate dogma (Timberg, 2007a; Timberg, 2007b;
Timberg, 2007c). Furthermore, inappropriate dogma (Smith, 2001) and confusion
about Koch’s postulates and causality in HIV/AIDS (Prusiner, 2002; cf. Smith,
2002/2007) contribute to scientific chaos and ultimately misplaced costs
(Smith, in preparation) Hence, a thoroughgoing analysis of the economics of
common sense and the economics of failures and aberrant common sense are
important matters for future investigation.
[94]
In terms of methodology and ethnomethodology, in situ phenomenological
studies are fraught
with many challenges. This study reveals the seminal failure of psychiatry and
the clinical
professions to respond to unknowing neediness in occasionally urgent situations
(such as, when
a person with aberrant common sense may be causing chaos and harm self or
others). There
also are enormous ethical, moral, economic, political and philosophy of science
challenges and
implications underlying this research. For brevity and succinctness, these
issues were not
addressed, but will be discussed elsewhere (Smith, in preparation).
[95]
Finally, the meta-transpersonal perspectives alluded to in the outline reveal a
need for listening,
observational, reasoning and anticipatory skills beyond the transpersonal.
References
(1)
Amos, D. (2007).
Young Iraqi Refuges in Syria Miss Schools, National Public Radio Morning
Edition (October 16, 2007; Accessible online at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15315081>).
Arbib, M. A.,
Billard, A., Iacoboni, M., and E. Oztop. (2000). Synthetic Brain Imaging:
Grasping, Mirror Neurons and Imitation. Neural Networks 13, 975-997.
Barker, D. J., Winter, P. D., Osmond, C., Margetts, B., and, Simmonds, S. J.
(1989). Weight in infancy and death from ischaemic heart disease. The Lancet
2:577-580. Barker, D. J. (2001). A new model for the origins of chronic
disease. Med Health Care Philosophy 4:31-35. Barker, D. J. (2002). Fetal
programming of coronary heart disease. Trends in Endocrinology and
Metabolism 13:364-368.
Barker, D. J. P.
(2003a). State-of-the-Art Lecture 1: Nutrition: impact on disease development.
Presented at 9th Asian Pacific Congress of Nephrology [February 16-20, 2003 –
Pattaya, THAILAND].
Barker, D. J.
(2003b). Coronary heart disease: a disorder of growth. Hormone Research
59 (Supplement 1):35-41.
Bihl, G. R. (2003).
Conference Report – Intrauterine Growth and Disease in Later Life, Medscape
Transplantation 4(1) [Highlights >From the 9th Asian Pacific Congress of
Nephrology; February 16-20, 2003; Pattaya, Thailand; accessible online at <http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/453242>].
Boccaccio, G.
(Circa 1330-1375/2000). Decameron. New York: Doubleday. ISBN-10: 0385000545;
ISBN-13: 978-0385000543.
Bourdieu, P.
(1998). Practical Reason: On the Theory of Action. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Braud, W., and
Anderson, R. (1998). Transpersonal Research Methods for the Social Sciences
– Honoring Human
Experience.
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing Up to the
Problem of Consciousness, Journal of Consciousness Studies 2(3),
200-219.
Cheam, S., and
Gordon, R. (2007). Hunger and Dance, The Story (National Public Radio) (Friday,
5 October 2007; accessible online at <http://thestory.org/archive/the_story_356_Hunger_And_Dance.mp3>).
Christakis, N. A.,
and Fowler, J. H. (2007). The Spread of Obesity in a Large Social Network over
32 Years, New England Journal of Medicine 357(4), 370-379. Coles, R.
(1967). A Study of Courage and Fear. New York: Atlantic Monthly Press.
ASIN: B000OKLXSE. Coles, R. (1977). Children of Crisis (Complete 5 Volume
Set): I. A Study of Courage & Fear II. Migrants, Sharecroppers,
Mountaineers III. South Goes North IV. Eskimos Chicanos Indians
V. Privileged
Ones.
Boston: Little Brown. ASIN: B000GZN2TK. Coles, R. (1986). The Political Life
of Children. New York: Atlantic Monthly Press. ISBN-10: 0871130351;
ISBN-13: 978-0871130358. Coles, R. (1989). The Call of Stories: Teaching and
the Moral Imagination. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN-10: 0395429358;
ISBN-13: 978-0395429358. Coles, R. (1990). The Spiritual Life of Children
(Children in Crisis). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN-10: 0395559995;
ISBN-13: 978-0395559994. Coles, R. (1995). The Story of Ruby Bridges.
New York: Scholastic Press. ISBN-10: 0590572814; ISBN-13: 978-0590572811.
Coles, R. (2000). The Moral Life of Children. New York: Atlantic Monthly
Press. ISBN-10: 0871137704; ISBN-13: 978-0871137708.
Coles, R. (2003). Children
of Crisis: Selections From The Pulitzer Prize-Winning Five-Volume Children of
Crisis Series. Boston: Back Bay Books. ISBN-10: 0316151025; ISBN-13:
9780316151023.
Dawkins, R. (1976).
The Selfish Gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Di Pellegrino, G.,
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor
events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research 91,
176–180.
DiPietro, J. A.,
Novak, M., Costigan, K., Atella, L., and Reusing, S. (2006). Maternal
Psychological Stress During Pregnancy in Relation to Child Development at Age
Two, Child Development 77(2), 502-503.
Dolinoy, D. C.,
Huang, D., and Jirtle, R. L. (2007). Maternal nutrition supplementation
counteracts bisphenol A-induced DNA hypomethylation in early development, Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(32):13056-13061.
Ennis, R. H.
(1984). Definition in science teaching, Instructional Science 3:
285-298.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Eriksson, J. G.,
Forsen, T., Tuomilehto, J., Osmond, C., and Barker, D. J. (2001). Early growth
and coronary heart disease in later life: longitudinal study. British
Medical Journal 322:94999453.
Fadiga, L.,
Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during
action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology
73, 2608–2611.
Forsen, T.,
Eriksson, J., Tuomilehto, J., Reunanen, A., Osmond, C., and Barker, D. (2000).
The fetal and childhood growth of persons who develop type 2 diabetes, Annals
of Internal Medicine 133:176-182.
Franco, Z., Pamlin,
D., Langdon, M., Blau, K., and Zimbardo, P. G. (2007). Is Lei Feng Still
a Modern Hero?: A Consideration of Heroic Action in the Context of Culture, Abstracts,
4th International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and
Psychotherapies [Baiyun Guangzhou, CHINA – September 24-26], pp. 21-24.
Fulghum, R
(1986/2004). All I Really Need to Know I Learned in Kindergarten – Uncommon
Thoughts on Common Things (also 15th Anniversary Revised Edition). New
York: Random House.
Gallese, V.,
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the
premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609.
Garfinkel, H.
(1967). Studies in Ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Gater, R.,
Tansella, M., Korten, A., Tiemens, B. G., Mavreas, V. G., and Olatawura, M. O.
(1998). Sex Differences in the Prevalence and Detection of Depressive and
Anxiety Disorders in General Health Care Settings: Report From the World Health
Organization Collaborative Study on Psychological Problems in General Health
Care, Archives of General Psychiatry 55:405-413.
Geertz, C. (1983). Local
Knowledge: Further Essays in Interpretive Anthropology. New York: Basic
Books.
Gettleman, J.
(2007). Rape Epidemic Raises Trauma of Congo War, New York Times (7 October
2007; accessible online at <http://www.nytimes.com/2007/10/07/world/africa/07congo.html?hp>).
Gladwell, M.
(2005). Blink – The Power of Thinking Without Thinking. New York:
Little, Brown and Company.
Godfrey, K. M., and
Barker, D. J. (2001). Fetal programming and adult health, Public Health
Nutrition 4:611-624.
Grafton, S. T.,
Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp
representations in humans by positron emission tomography. 2. Observation
compared with imagination. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 103–111.
Haig, D. (1996).
Altercation of generations: genetic conflicts of pregnancy, American Journal
of Reproductive Immunology 35:226-232.
Handel, W. (1982). Ethnomethodology:
How People Make Sense. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hersey, J. C.,
Klibanoff, L. S., Lam, D. J., and Taylor, R. L. (1984). Promoting Social
Support: The Impact of California's "Friends Can be Good Medicine"
Campaign, Health Education & Behavior 11(3), 293-311 [DOI:
10.1177/109019818401100306].
Hoehn-Saric, R.,
McLeod, D. R., Funderburk, F., and Kowalski, P. (2004). Somatic Symptoms and
Physiologic Responses in Generalized Anxiety Disorder and Panic Disorders: An
Ambulatory Monitor Study, Archives of General Psychiatry 61:913-921.
Horrobin, D. F. (1982).
Peer review: is the good the enemy of the best? Journal of Research
Communication Studies 3(4):327-34.
Hunter, L., and
Lloyd-Kolkin, D. (1983). Friends Can Be Good Medicine: Educating the Community
about Social Support and Health. Paper presented at the 67th Annual Meeting
of the American Educational Research Association (Montreal, CANADA – April
11-14).
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Iacoboni, M. et al.
(1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286, 2526–2528.
Iacoboni, M. et al.
(2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal
cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 98,
13995–13999.
Iacoboni, M.,
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J.C., and Rizzolatti,
G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron
system. PloS Biology 3(3), E79, pp. 0529-0535.
Keller, H. (1961). The
Story of My Life. New York: Dell Books (Mass Market Paperbacks). ASIN:
B000KEJNSG. [Also memorialized in the 2001 film The Miracle Worker by
Anne Bancroft, Victor Jory, Inga Swenson, and Andrew Prine.]
Kelley, C. F.,
Barbour, J. D., and Hecht, F. M. (2007). The Relation Between Symptoms, Viral
Load (VL), and Viral Load Set Point in Primary HIV Infection (PHI), Journal
of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome 45(4):445-448 (also accessible
online at <http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/562500>).
Kestenbaum, D.
(2007). Adaptation & Survival - Mottainai Grandma Reminds Japan,
'Don't Waste', National Public Radio’s Morning Edition (8 October 2007;
accessible online at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=14054262>).
Killian, J. K.,
Byrd, J. C., Jirtle, J. V., Munday, B. L., Stoskopf, M. K., MacDonald, R. G.,
and Jirtle, R. L., (2000). M6P/IGF2R imprinting evolution in mammals, Molecular
Cell 5:707-716.
Klevens, R. M.,
Morrison, M. A., Nadle, J., Petit, S., Gershman, K., Ray, S., Harrison, L. H.,
Lynfield, R., Dumyati, G., Townes, J. M., Craig, A. S., Zell, E. R., Fosheim,
G. E., McDougal,
L. K., Carey, R.
B., Fridkin, S. K., and for the Active Bacterial Core surveillance (ABCs) MRSA
Investigators (2007). Invasive Methicillin-Resistant Staphylococcus aureus
Infections in the United States, Journal of the American Medical Association
298(15):1763-1771.
Kuhn, T. S.
(1962/1996). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Lakatos, I.
(1965/1970). Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. Proceedings of the
International Colloquium in the Philosophy of Science, London, 1965.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lakatos, I. (1978).
The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Lesser, L. I.,
Ebbeling, C. B., Goozner, M., Wypij, D., and Ludwig, D. S. (2007). Relationship
between Funding Source and Conclusion among Nutrition-Related Scientific
Articles, PloS Medicine 4(1), 1-6.
McClintock, B.
(1950). The Origin and behavior of mutable loci in maize. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences (USA) 36:344-355.
Milliken, C. S.,
Auchterlonie, J. L., and Hoge, C. W. (2007). Longitudinal Assessment of Mental
Health Problems Among Active and Reserve Component Soldiers Returning From the
Iraq War, Journal of the American Medical Association 298(18):2141-2148.
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders
of Personality: DSM III, Axis II. New York: Wiley-Interscience (John Wiley
and Sons, Inc.), see especially part 2, chapters 4-11 (pp 107-324).
Montagne, R.
(2007). The Impact of War: War Comes Home for Mother-Soldiers (2nd in a 3 part
series), National Public Radio Morning Edition (November 13, 2007;
accessible online at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=16237067>).
Moss, N.J., Harper,
C. C., Ahrens, K., Scott, K., Kao, S., Padian, N., Raine, T., and Klausner, J.
D. (2007). Predictors of incident herpes simplex
virus type 2 infections in young women at risk for unintended pregnancy in San
Francisco, BioMedCentral (BMC) Infectious Diseases
7:113 DOI:
10.1186/1471-2334-7-113.
Mullooly, J.
(2003). Work, Play And Consequences: What Counts In A Successful Middle School.
New York: Columbia University, Ph.D. Dissertation, School of Arts and
Sciences.
Mullooly, J.
(2006). “Un”common Sense: Shifting Perspectives in the Anthropological
Investigation of Cognition, Public Lecture, California State University
at Fresno, February 3.
Nagel, T. 1974.
What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 4:435-50.
Norris, M. (2007).
Women in Combat: On the Ground in Iraq, National Public Radio - All Things
Considered (2 October 2007 accessed at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=14901578>).
Norris, M., and
Hillard, G. (2007). Women in Combat: Reported Cases of Sexual Assault in
Military Rise - Post-Traumatic Stress Syndrome, Sexual Harassment in the
Military, National Public Radio - All Things Considered (4 October 2007
accessed online at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15005484>).
Owen, W. (1965). The
Collected Works of Wilfred Owen. New York: New Directions Publishing Corporation;
Revised edition. ISBN-10: 0811201325; ISBN-13: 978-0811201322.
Patchen, K., with
Smith, L. (1991). Awash With Roses: The Collected Love Poems of Kenneth
Patchen Huron, OH: Bottom Dog Press, p.154 [ISBN: 978-0933087217].
Pauling, L., Itano,
H. A., Singer, S. J., and Wells, I. C. (1949). Sickle Cell Anemia, a Molecular
Disease, Science 110: 543-548 [DOI:
10.1126/science.110.2865.543].Reuters (2006). Teens Suffer Soap Opera Virus.
May 18, 2006 news release <http://go.reuters.com/newsArticle.jhtml?type=oddlyEnoughNews&storyID=12254563&src=e
Dialog/GetContent>.
Platt, J. R.
(1964). Strong inference, Science 146(3642):347-353.
Popper, K. R.
(1963). Conjectures and Refutations. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Prusiner, S. B.
(2002). “Discovering the Cause of AIDS,” Science 298(5599):1726-1727.
[DOI: 10.1126/science.1079874].
Ramachandran, V. S.
(2000). MIRROR NEURONS and imitation learning as the driving force behind
"the great leap forward" in human evolution. Edge <http://www.edge.org/documents/archive/edge69.html>.
Reik, W., and
Walter, J. (2001). Genomic imprinting: parental influence on the genome, Nature
Reviews in Genetics 2: 21-32.
Ries, R [Consensus
Panel Chair]. (1996). Assessment and Treatment of Patients With Coexisting
Mental Illness and Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse, Treatment Improvement Protocol
Series 9, Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, Public
Health Service, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 5600
Fishers Lane, Rockville, MD 20857; Chapter 6 – Anxiety Disorders, pp43-51.
Robinson, P.
(1983). From Suttee To Baseball To Cockfighting, New York Times (25
September 1983; accessible online at <http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9F05E1DF1E38F936A1575AC0A96594826
0&sec=&spon=&pagewanted=all>).
Sabatier, R.
(1988). Blaming Others: Prejudice, Race and Worldwide AIDS. London: The PANOS
Institute.
Sakano, H., Huppi,
K., Heinrich, G., and Tonegawa, S. (1979). Sequences at the recombination sites
of immunoglobulin light-chain genes. Nature 280:288-294.
Schlosberg, J., and
Budzinski, D. (2004). The Battle at Kruger, YouTube videoclip
(accessible online at <http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=LU8DDYz68kM>). [Source material
available at <http://www.negativespace.com/contact.htm>].
Seddon, K. H. (date
unknown). Lao Tzu – Tao Te Ching. London: BM Box 1129, London WC1N 3XX,
United Kingdom. [Entire text available online at <www.btinternet.com/~k.h.s/ttc.htm>.]
Shinju, M.
(2004/2005). Mottainai Baasan [Mottainai Grandma]42. Tokyo: Koudansha
(shop.kodansha.jp; also see <http://www.mottainai.com/>).
42 A translation
for the Japanese word “Mottainai” is “what a waste!” or “do not waste” or
“waste not; want not.” Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All
rights reserved.
Simon, H. A. (1945ff).
Administrative Behavior: A Study of Decision-Making Processes in
Administrative Organizations. New York: The Free Press (Simon and Shuster).
Smith, R. W.
(1971). Compilers with common sense, unpublished paper, University of
California, Santa Barbara, CA.
Smith, R. W.
(1979). Long-Term Memories: Where Does the 'Buck' Stop? — Toward a Testable
Theory of Debugging the Molecular Basis of Long-Term Memories in Living
Organisms. Abstracts, Seventh Meeting of the International Society for
Neurochemistry [Jerusalem, ISRAEL — September 2-6], p. 590 (also see Exhibit 1
in this report).
Smith, R. W.
(1983). How Would You Recognize and Detect a Virally Modified Code? Could Kuru
be Caused by Cannibalism of Autotoxic Factors in Brain and Lymphoid Tissue?
Does Purification of Molecular Function Differ from Purification of Molecular
Structure? – A Critique of Impure Reasoning in Biological Sciences. Abstracts,
7th International Congress of Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science
[Salzburg, AUSTRIA — July] 4:358-362.
Smith, R. W.
(1984). AIDS and 'Slow Viruses', Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 437:576-607.
Smith, R. W.
(1986). The National Impact of Negativistic Leadership: A Need for National
Caveats Emptor (unpublished book outline). Palo Alto, CA: Institute for
Postgraduate Interdisciplinary Studies.
Smith, R. W.
(1987). The National Impact of Negativistic Leadership: A Need for National
Caveats Emptor. In Abstracts, 1987 Annual Meeting of the Western
Political Science Association [Anaheim, CA — March 26-28], p. 28.
Smith, R. W.
(1988). Transmissible Negativism and Its Possible Relation to Irrational
Behavior and Poor Common Sense. Presented to the XXIV International Congress
of Psychology [Sidney, AUSTRALIA — August 28 - September 2].
Smith, R. W.
(1989). Why is AIDS Important to VTSS [Values-Technology-Science-Society]? Techné
(Stanford University Publication) III:27-39.
Smith, R. W.
(1992). The National Impact of Negativistic Leadership: A Need for National
Caveats Emptor (final draft of book outline). Institute for Postgraduate
Interdisciplinary Studies, Palo Alto, CA. [Self-published, May 12, 1992].
Smith, R. W.
(1994). On Mechanisms of Slowness and Progressiveness in Slowly Progressive
Processes, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 476: 430-434.
Smith, R. W.
(2001). Opinion: The Durban Declaration, The Scientist 15(2):39 [see www.thescientist.com/yr2001/jan/opin_010122.html].
Smith, R. Wm.
(2002). On the Biogenesis and Development of Terrorism. Web-telecast
presentation to The San Francisco Tesla Society <http://sftesla.org/Newsletters/2002/january2002.htm>
[San Francisco, CA – January 13].
Smith, R. W.
(2002/2007). On Causality in AIDS, Unpublished (rejected by Science).
Smith, R. Wm.
(2003a). Autovirulence – An epigenetic model of molecular mimicry in living
systems: Broad implications for aberrant translation products and autoimmunity.
Abstracts, XIX International Congress of Genetics [Melbourne, AUSTRALIA
– July 6-11], Abstract #2.G.0246, p. 225.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2003b). Revisiting the Molecular Biology, Genetics and Genomics of Long-Term
Memory in Living Systems. Abstracts, XIX International Congress of
Genetics [Melbourne, AUSTRALIA – July 6-11], Abstract #5.C.0802, p. 133.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2004a). Towards a Molecular Biology of Long-Term Memory in Humans:
Implications for Research on Consciousness, Subconsciousness, Dreaming, Nurture
and Commonsense – a poster presentation. Consciousness Research Abstracts,
Towards a Science of Consciousness 2004 Conference [Tucson, AZ – February
6-11], Session P1 (Abstract #196), p. 80.
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2004b). Frontier Models in Transpersonal Research & Holistic Practice –
Molecular Models of Transpersonal Processes: Nature, Nurture and Psychosomatic
Illness, Abstracts, 112th Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association [Honolulu, HI
– July 31], Session
#4195, p. 334.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2004c). Commonsense, Aberrant Commonsense and Transmissible Negativism: Developmental
Effects of ‘Psychoviruses’. Abstracts, 112th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association [Honolulu, HI – August 1], Session
#5142, p. 389.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2005a). Toward a Molecular and Genomic Basis for the Transpersonal and Its
Evolution. In Abstracts, Conference of European Transpersonal
Associations (EUROTAS) 2005 [Moscow, RUSSIA – June 24]
Smith, R. Wm.
(2005b). A Novel and Testable Model of Post-Darwinian Evolution: Implications
for Proteomics in Speciation and Genetics, and Non-Proteomics in Nurturance and
the Transpersonal. In Abstracts, 3rd International Conference on
Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychology [Beijing, CHINA – September 26], p. 4.
Smith, R. Wm.
(2006a). Evolution and Long-Term Memories in Living Systems: Using molecular
biology to resolve three great debates … Lamarck versus Darwin, Nature versus
Nurture, and The Central Dogma. Presentation to the San Francisco Tesla
Society (<http://www.sftesla.org/Newsletters/newslett2006.htm>) [San Francisco, CA –
February 12].
Smith, R. Wm.
(2006b). Revisiting and Rethinking Evolution: Implications for Substantive
Professionalization of General Education. Presented to the 18th
International Conference on Systems Research, Informatics and Cybernetics
(InterSymp 2006) “Axiomatic Education – IV, The 4th Symposium on the
Substantive Professionalization of General Education.” [Baden-Baden, GERMANY –
August 9].
Smith, R. Wm.
(2007a). On the Biogenesis, Development and Evolution of Common Sense and
Spirituality: Implications for Critical Reasoning in the Sciences, Religions
and Society. A presentation to the San Francisco Tesla Society [San Francisco –
January 14th] (see <http://www.sftesla.org/Newsletters/2007/SFTS_news_2007_01_14.htm>).
Smith, R. Wm.
(2007b). On the Biogenesis, Development and Evolution of Common Sense and
Spirituality: Implications for Critical Reasoning in the Sciences, Religions
and Society, Abstracts, 15th Annual Winter Chaos Conference (The
Snowflake Forum): Dynamical Systems Thinking in Science, Education, and Society
[San Juan and Arecibo, PUERTO RICO – March 29-April 2] (see <http://www.blueberrybrain.org/winterchaos/snowflake2007.html#roulette>).
Smith, R. Wm.
(2007c). Early Childhood Development: Roles for Natural Instances of Chaos in
the Evolution and Development of Common Sense in Humans, Abstracts, 17th
Annual International Conference, Society for Chaos Theory in Psychology &
Life Sciences [Orange, CA – July 27-29] (see <http://www.societyforchaostheory.org/conf2007/>).
Smith, R. Wm.
(2007d). For Common Sense’s Sake, Give Peace A Chance! Presented to a Special
Workshop on Applying Complexity Science to Peacebuilding and Conflict Analysis
at the Fletcher School, Tufts University, Medford, MA 02155 USA (August 10-11,
2007).
Smith, R. Wm.
(2007e). Failures in Gene Therapies and HIV Vaccine Research – Toward a
Parsimonious Theory of Virus-Related Infectious Cellular Substituents, AIDS
Vaccines, and ‘Proof’ Theory in Life Sciences. Rejected by the New England
Journal of Medicine and The Lancet. Available from the author at
<najms@postgraduate-interdisciplinary-studies.org>.
Smith, R. Wm (in
preparation). Revisiting and Rethinking Evolution: Implications for
Substantive Professionalization of General Education. Palo Alto, CA:
Institute for Postgraduate Interdisciplinary Studies. [This is a greatly
expanded version of a 2006 paper with the same title.].
Taormina, R.
(2006). Cultural Differences in Psychological Reactions to Imposed Restrictions
on Individual Freedom. Presented at Forum 2006 – On Individual Freedom and
Individual Responsibility: Balancing Society’s Requirements and Individual
Needs [InterSymp -2006 Program of the 18th International Conference on
Systems Research, Informatics and Cybernetics Baden-Baden, GERMANY – August
10.]
Taylor, R. L., Lam,
D. J., Roppel, C. E., and Barter, J. T. (2004). Friends Can Be Good Medicine:
An excursion into mental health promotion, Behavioral Science 20(4),
294-303 [DOI: 10.1007/BF00757078].
Timberg, C.
(2007a). Warning Is Sent to AIDS Vaccine Volunteers: S. Africans Among
Recipients Who May Be at Higher Risk of Contracting Virus, Washington Post
October 25, 2007; page A20 (also accessible online at <http://www.washingtonpost.com/wpdyn/content/article/2007/10/24/AR2007102402514.html>).
Timberg, C. (2007b).
Rethinking AIDS Strategy After a String of Failures: In Wake of Canceled
Vaccine Study, Some Experts Are Reemphasizing Proven, Low-Tech Prevention
Methods, Washington Post October 25, 2007; page A12 (also accessible
online at <http://www.washingtonpost.com/wpdyn/content/article/2007/10/31/AR2007103102919.html>).
Timberg, C.
(2007c). Failure of AIDS Shot Linked to Cold Virus, Washington Post
November 8, 2007; page A24 (accessible online at <http://www.washingtonpost.com/wpdyn/content/article/2007/11/07/AR2007110702466.html?sub=AR>).
Tonegawa, S.,
Maxam, A. M., Tizard, R., et al. (1978). Sequence of a mouse germ-line gene for
a variable region of an immunoglobulin light chain. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences (USA) 75:1485-1489.
Tuchman, B. (1978).
A Distant Mirror – The Calamitous 14th Century. New York: Knopf. ASIN:
B000IOUCO2.
Umiuchi, I., Amano,
Iwabashi, Murakami, Yamamoto et al. (Editors). (1967). SHOGAKU DŌU
TŌKU/どうとく (Morality / The way of
virtue) for 1st, 2nd, and 3rd grades. Osaka –shi, Japan: Osaka Shoseki
Inc., 2-1-1, Fukae Kita, Higashinari-Ku, Osaka-Shi 537, JAPAN. (<www.osaka-shoseki.co.jp>).
Verny, T. R., and
Kelly, J. (1981/1983). Das Seelenleben des Ungeborenen – Wie Mütter und Väter
schon fur der Geburt Persönlichkeit und Glück ihres Kindes fördern können [The
Soul Life of the Unborn Child …, sometimes translated as The Secret Life
of the Unborn Child …]. Berlin: Rogner & Bernhard.
Verny, T. R., and
Weintraub, P. (2002). Pre-Parenting: Nurturing Your Child From Conception [previously
published as Tomorrow’s Baby]. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Wilson, B. (2007).
HIV Education Lacking at Many College Orientations, National Public Radio Weekend
Edition Sunday (October 7, 2007; accessible online at <http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15080041>).
Wu, Y. (1989). The
Book Of Lao Tzu (the Tao Te Ching). San Francisco: Great Learning
Publishing Company, Chapters 29, 30, 32, and 44.
Yamamoto, M.,
Kamidera, H., Morita, T., Maeda, K., Kodera, M., Ookubo, S., Uwadera, and 5
other persons (Editors) (unknown date, though after 1980). IKIRU CHIKARA/いきる
ちから ( Living Force/Power or Power/Force to Live)
for 1st, 2nd, and 3rd Grades. Osaka –shi, Japan: Osaka Shoseki Kabashiki
Kaisha (Gaisha) Inc., 2-1-1, Fukae Kita, Higashinari-Ku, Osaka-Shi 537, JAPAN.
(<www.osaka-shoseki.co.jp>).
Zhang, J. (2007).
Pictorial Image Expression Therapy and Literary Guidance, Abstracts, 4th
International Conference on Humanistic and Transpersonal Psychologies and
Psychotherapies [Baiyun Guangzhou, CHINA – September 24-26].
Zimbardo, P. G.
(2006). The banality of evil is matched by the banality of heroism, Edge
(accessed online at <http://www.edge.org/q2006/q06_index.html#zimbardo>).
Endnote
[96] Help for
Unknowingly Needy and Worried Well – A paradigm and model for social design
and engineering
It is said that …
“Mankind may be divided into four classes:
(1) |
Those who KNOW and know that they KNOW – of them seek knowledge; |
(2) |
Those who KNOW but do not know that they KNOW – awaken them; |
(3) |
Those that do not KNOW and know that they do not KNOW – instruct them; |
(4) |
Those who do not KNOW but think that they KNOW – they are fools, dismiss |
|
them.” |
|
Salomon Ibn Gabirol (also known as Avicebron) |
|
In Mibhar Hu-Peninim [Choice of Pearls] |
|
No. 60 (circa 1050 AD) |
[97] |
|
An analysis of this
adage reveals that those with and without knowledge may be partitioned, albeit
somewhat simplistically, according to their education, alertness, motivation
and ability to educate. The range of possibilities is even more instructive if
the words “NEED” and/or “HELP” are substituted for the
capitalized and italicized word “KNOW.”
Acknowledgments
[98] No research is
performed in a vacuum! Many contributors often are unaware of their profound
contributions – even in seemingly insignificant ways. Thus, it is a great honor
and pleasure to acknowledge seminal insights and contributions from the
following persons, though not in any specific order: Norma Abe; Yvonne Allen;
Can Altinbay; Professor Greg Andonian; Professor Mark Arons; Henry and Sue
Bass; Ann Bromley; David Brownell, Ph.D.; Professor Jerry L. R. Chandler; J.
Andrew Chapman; John Clemens; Angelique Delaney; Stephanie Delaney; Neill
Edwards; Michael Eisenstadt; Glenda Eoyang, Ph.D.; Ms Hirose, the school
librarian at Cherry Chase Elementary School in Sunnyvale, CA; Professor Douglas
Hofstadter; Professor Stanley Krippner; Herr Klaus Krückemeyer; Karla Bindokat,
Ph.D.; Ursula Lemcke-Schmalzl, Esq.; Caesar Freiherr von Paleske, Esq.;
Professor Ming Lee; Ricki A. Lewis, Ph.D.; Lin Ling “Eleanor” Luo; Giancarlo
Magnago; Mathematics Laboratory students in 1985 classes at the Cherry Chase
Elementary School in Sunnyvale, CA (USA); Alan Mease, M.D.; Byron Marshall;
Professor James Mullooly; Yoko Noda; Valdus Plato; Professor Henry Poon; Vivian
Powers; The Very Reverend Scott Richardson; Aneetha “Anita” Rodriquez; Salamin
Alphonse; Dave Schmalz; Nicole M. Smith, Ph.D.; Todd R. Smith; Mollie Spring
(pseudonym); Professor James Stasheff; Janet Dickerson Stephens, L.L.D. [Hon.
Caus.]; J. Paul Stephens, Ed.D.; David Sutton, Ph.D.; Students at an evening
forum at the Guangzhou University of Chinese Medicine; Tajiri Takanari; Jutta
Thompson; Margaret S. Warner, Ph.D.; Professor Jerry Unger, Julio Vidaurrazaga;
Ram Lakhan Pandey Vimal, Ph.D.; Professor Gerard De Zeeuw; Jiazhen Zhang; Cai
Fang “Jeremy” Zhu; Professor Fei Zi; and, not least, co-researchers “A,” “B,”
“C,” “D,” “E,” “F,” “W,” “X,” “Y,” and “Z.” Perhaps most significant, I am
grateful to Professors Cyrus and Magdalena Lee for providing an opportunity to
present and explicate another unusual example of the transpersonal (i.e.,
common sense) along with its clinical implications (i.e., aberrant common
sense, unknowing neediness, and worried wellness).
Table 1 – Examples of the Term “Common Sense” in
Different Languages
• 常識 traditional43 or 常识
simplified (“chang shi”; Chinese)
• 常識 (jyoshiki; Japanese – common
sense)
• 良識 (ryoshiki; Japanese – good sense)
• 상식 (Korean)
• trí-thưć phô’-thông (Vietnamese –
pronounced “tree tuck foe tom”)
• สามัานก (Thai – pronounced “saaRM”
or “sahmR neukH;” common sense)44
ญสํึ
man
43 There is some
evidence that these idiographic Chinese characters may date back several
thousand years. Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights
reserved.
44 Every syllable
in Thai is pronounced in one of five tones: low, mid, high, falling, or rising.
The transliteration shown in the dictionary indicates the required tone with a
superscripted capital letter after the syllable; L, M, H, F, or R,
respectively.45 “There is no phrase in Plato that expresses what we mean
nowadays by common sense. There may be a phrase in Aristotle equivalent to our
meaning of common sense, although we are unable to identify that term in his
writings. This may be due to limitations on available search engines. Nor does
an equivalent expression for common sense appear in the Septuagint, the Greek
translation of the bible. The term does appear in the Latin of Cicero who uses
the phrase in the modern meaning of the phrase” (Michael Eisenstadt, personal
communications).46 It is possible that this term may date back thousands of
years (circa Manu’s formulation of the caste system in India), although we are
not able to confirm this hypothesis. The term “Aprikshitakaraka” certainly is
known to exist in the Panchatantra – the “five principles.” It is the fifth of
the five principles, and, roughly translated, means “knowledge is not enough,
application is essential.”
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Table 2 –
Psychological and Physical Symptoms in the Propositi in this Study
Propositi Psychological |
“A” |
“B” |
“C” |
“D” |
“E” |
“F” |
“X” |
“Y” |
|
“Z” |
Gender |
F |
F |
F |
F |
M |
F |
M |
F |
|
F |
Aberrant common sense |
XXX |
|
XXX X |
XXX XX |
XX |
X |
X |
XX |
|
X |
Impaired reasoning, anticipatory & problem-solving skills |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX X |
XXX |
X |
|
XX |
|
X |
Negativism |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX |
|
|
X |
X |
|
unk |
Selfish |
XXX |
|
XXX X |
XXX X |
unk |
X |
X+ |
unk |
|
unk |
Witty & humorous “zingers” |
XX |
|
XX |
X |
|
XX |
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
Unconditional love for offspring |
XXX |
N/A |
N/A |
XXX |
N/A |
X |
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
Poor parenting skills |
XXX XX |
N/A |
N/A |
XXX XX |
N/A |
|
XXX |
unk |
|
unk |
Blames others |
XXX |
|
XX |
XX |
unk |
X |
X |
X |
|
unk |
Mood swings |
XXX X |
|
XX |
XXX |
X |
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
Obsessive-Compulsive |
|
XX |
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
X |
|
unk |
Passive-aggressive, tardy & procrastinates |
XXX |
|
XXX XXX |
XXX XX |
? |
|
XXX X |
unk |
|
unk |
Internecine behavior |
X |
|
XXX |
|
|
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
Suicidal ideation |
X |
|
unk |
XXX |
unk |
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
‘My way or the highway’ |
XXX X |
|
XXX |
XXX X |
unk |
X |
X |
XX |
|
X |
Exhibitionist |
X |
|
X |
|
|
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
High risk sexual behavior |
XX |
X |
X |
XX |
|
X |
XX |
unk |
|
unk |
Constantly seeking love |
XX |
? |
XXX X |
XXX X |
N/A |
X |
X |
|
|
|
Interracial, interfaith, interethnic relationships |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX X |
N/A |
X |
XXX |
unk |
|
unk |
High failure rate in relationships |
XXX |
X |
XXX X |
XXX XXX |
? |
X |
X |
unk |
|
unk |
Addiction |
X |
|
|
|
N/A |
|
XX |
unk |
|
unk |
Teetotaler |
|
|
X |
X |
N/A |
|
|
unk |
|
unk |
Aberrant distillation skills |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX X |
? |
|
X |
X |
|
X |
• Impractical concerns and/or poor knowledge |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX X |
|
|
X+ |
X |
|
X |
• Insensitivities |
XX |
|
X |
XXX |
|
|
XX |
X |
|
X |
• Poor discernment |
XX |
|
|
XXX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
|
unk |
• Poor mindfulness |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX |
|
|
X |
X |
|
X |
|
|
|
|
X |
|
|
|
|
|
Prone to mistakes, breakages & errors |
XXX |
|
X |
XX |
|
X |
unk |
unk |
unk |
Misunderstandings |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX |
|
X |
unk |
unk |
unk |
Does not accept responsibility |
XXX |
|
XXX |
XXX XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Difficulties in understanding |
XXX XX |
|
|
XXX XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Chaos & unbelievably thoughtless (Footnote 31) |
XXX X |
|
X |
XXX |
|
|
XX |
unk |
unk |
Confuses reasons & excuses |
XXX |
|
X |
XX |
|
|
X |
unk |
unk |
Inappropriate body language |
XX |
|
X |
|
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Narcissistic and single-minded |
X |
|
XX |
XX |
|
|
X |
unk |
unk |
Savant-like behavior |
|
|
X |
|
X |
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Irrational actions, fears, worries & risks |
XXX X |
|
XX |
XXX XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Interest in occult and |
XX |
|
|
XXX |
|
X |
unk |
unk |
X |
mystical |
|
|
|
X |
|
|
|
|
|
Physical and Somatic |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Anxiety > 6 months & generalized anxieties |
XXX X |
|
XX |
XXX XXX |
X |
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Medicated for anxiety |
Yes |
|
|
No |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Rapid heat beat |
XX |
|
|
XXX |
|
|
X |
unk |
unk |
Sweating |
X |
|
|
XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Difficulty breathing |
X |
|
|
XXX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Feeling tense |
XX |
|
X |
XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Muscle tension |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Neck and back pains |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Headaches |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Trembling |
X |
|
|
XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Irritability |
XX |
|
X |
X |
|
|
X |
unk |
unk |
Fatigue |
X |
|
X |
XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Easily “stressed out” |
XXX |
|
XX |
XXX |
unk |
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Stress-induced (EBVrelated) epigenetic disorders |
X |
|
? |
XX |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Psychosomatic disorders |
XXX |
|
X |
XXX X |
|
|
unk |
unk |
unk |
Copyright © 2007 by
Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. – All rights reserved.
Exhibit 1
Term Memories
and “Immune Dementia” Were Proposed
Abstracts, 7th MEETING OF THE
INTERNATIONAL SOCIETY
FOR NEUROCHEMISTRY, Jerusalem, ISRAEL - September 2-6, 1979, p. 590.
"LONG-TERM
MEMORIES: Where Does The 'Buck' Stop? – Toward
a Testable Theory of Debugging the Molecular Basis of Long
Term Memories in Living Organisms"
Roulette Wm. SMITH,
PhD
School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco
Editor – INSTRUCTIONAL SCIENCE & HEALTH POLICY AND EDUCATION
A review of the
literature on the molecular basis of memory
reveals no evidence supporting extant views that RNA and/or
proteins may be the repository of long-term memories (LTM)
in living organisms. This study reveals a plausible and
testable model based on DNA modifications, insertions, trans-
positions and reverse transcriptions which is more consistent
with published reports. The theory may explain the "slow-
ness" of slowly transmissible infectious agents, certain can-
cers, and a variety of other clinical entities. Independent
ly of the accuracy of the theory of LTM, we show that no mod-
el of LTM can be complete without the definitive evaluation
of DNA as the ultimate repository of information. This the-
sis enables one to discuss the evolutionary and development-
al basis of memory, and to describe experimental technologies
considered essential for the explication of memory. The
study also reveals the potential for studying memory at the
cellular level with differentiation being analogous to LTM
and autophagy/cellular optimization being more analogous to
STM. Implications for a variety of multidisciplinary issues
are described, including aging, evolution, DNA dynamics,
virology, experimental design, theory formulation and the
philosophy of science, and, the search for the elusive engram
in psychology.
Exhibit
2
Pictorial Essay from Benetton Magazine Special Issue on AIDS
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Exhibit
3 – An Example of a “Friends can be good medicine” Poster
Transpersonal Aspects of Helping Persons With
Aberrant Common Sense 61
Exhibit 4 –Examples of Elementary Moral and Common Sense Education from a Japanese Textbook
Copyright © 2007 by Roulette William Smith, Ph.D. –
All rights reserved.
|
|